%0 Journal Article %A Pagel, Mark %A Beaumont, Mark %A Meade, Andrew %A Verkerk, Annemarie %A Calude, Andreea %+ Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Max Planck Society %T Dominant words rise to the top by positive frequency-dependent selection : %G eng %U https://hdl.handle.net/21.11116/0000-0007-7C45-1 %R 10.1073/pnas.1816994116 %F OTHER: shh1204 %7 2019-03-21 %D 2019 %8 09.04.2019 %* Review method: peer-reviewed %X Speakers of a language somehow come to use the same words to express particular meanings—}like {“}dog{”} or {“}table{”}{—}even though there is seldom a necessary connection between a word and its meaning, and there are often many alternatives from which to choose (e.g., {“}sofa,{”} {“}couch,{”} {“}settee{”}). We show that word choice is not just a matter of saying what others say. Rather, humans seem to be equipped with a bias that makes them disproportionately more likely to use the words that most others use. The force of this bias can drive competing words out, allowing a single word to dominate all others. It can also explain how languages spontaneously organize and remain relatively stable for centuries or even millennia.A puzzle of language is how speakers come to use the same words for particular meanings, given that there are often many competing alternatives (e.g., {“}sofa,{”} {“}couch,{”} {“}settee{”}), and there is seldom a necessary connection between a word and its meaning. The well-known process of random drift{—}roughly corresponding in this context to {“}say what you hear{”}{—}can cause the frequencies of alternative words to fluctuate over time, and it is even possible for one of the words to replace all others, without any form of selection being involved. However, is drift alone an adequate explanation of a shared vocabulary? Darwin thought not. Here, we apply models of neutral drift, directional selection, and positive frequency-dependent selection to explain over 417,000 word-use choices for 418 meanings in two natural populations of speakers. We find that neutral drift does not in general explain word use. Instead, some form of selection governs word choice in over 91% of the meanings we studied. In cases where one word dominates all others for a particular meaning{—}such as is typical of the words in the core lexicon of a language{—}word choice is guided by positive frequency-dependent selection{—a bias that makes speakers disproportionately likely to use the words that most others use. This bias grants an increasing advantage to the common form as it becomes more popular and provides a mechanism to explain how a shared vocabulary can spontaneously self-organize and then be maintained for centuries or even millennia, despite new words continually entering the lexicon. %K language evolution, neutral drift, frequency-dependent selection, shared vocabulary, approximate Bayesian computation %J Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America %V 116 %N 15 %& 7397 %P 7397 - 7402 %I National Academy of Sciences %C Washington, D.C. %@ 0027-8424