Publikationen
| 2025 | 2024 | 2023 | 2022 | 2021 | 2020 | 2019 | 2018 | 2017 | 2016 | 2015 | 2014 | 2013 | 2012 | 2011 | 2010 | 2009 | 2008 | 2007 | 2006 | 2005 | 2004 | 2003 | 2002 | 2001
Im Druck
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Social cognition in the second year of life. In A. Leslie, & T. German ( |
2025
Tomasello, M. (2025). Agency and Intentionality. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 62: 101501. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2025). Children's developing understanding of social norms (advance online). Current Opinion in Psychology, 64: 102022. |
|
Wolf, W., & Tomasello, M. (2025). A shared intentionality account of uniquely human social bonding. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 20(2), 264-275. |
2024
Benozio, A., House, B. R., & Tomasello, M. (2024). Gender and cultural differences in the development of reciprocity in young children. Developmental Psychology, 60, 1082-1096. |
|
Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2024). Instrumental helping motivations of children and chimpanzees (advance online). Child Development Perspectives. |
|
Kaminski, J., Stengelin, R., Girndt, A., Haun, D., & Liebal, K. (2024). Understanding others' preferences: A comparison across primate species and human societies. PLoS ONE, 19(1): e0295221. |
|
Knofe, H., Engelmann, J. M., Grueneisen, S., & Herrmann, E. (2024). Instrumental helping and short‐term reciprocity in chimpanzees and human children. Ethology, 130(4): e13426. |
|
Salas-Morellón, L., Palacios-Huerta, I., & Call, J. (2024). Dynamic inconsistency in great apes. Scientific Reports, 14: 18130. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2024). An agency-based model of executive and metacognitive regulation. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 2. |
|
Vasil, J., Capoot, C., & Tomasello, M. (2024). Effects of group entitativity on young English-speaking children’s interpretation of inclusive We. PLoS One, 19: e0306556. |
|
Vasil, J., Price, D., & Tomasello, M. (2024). Thought and language: Effects of group‐mindedness on young children's interpretation of exclusive we. Child Development, 95(3): e155–e163, pp. 657-1040. |
|
Vasil, J., Provençal, M., & Tomasello, M. (2024). Effects of “We”-framing and partner number on 2- and 3-year-olds’ sense of commitment. Cognitive Development, 72: 101511. |
|
Winter, P., & Tomasello, M. (2024). From what I want to do to what we decided to do: 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, honor their agreements with peers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 239: 105811. |
2023
Altınok, N., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2023). Young children use imitation communicatively. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 231: 105654. |
|
Benozio, A., House, B. R., & Tomasello, M. (2023). Apes reciprocate food positively and negatively. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 290(1998): 20222541. |
|
Bohn, M., Tessler, M. H., Kordt, C., Hausmann, T., & Frank, M. C. (2023). An individual differences perspective on pragmatic abilities in the preschool years. Developmental Science, 26(6): e13401. |
|
Colle, L., Grosse, G., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2023). Just teasing! - Infants' and toddlers' understanding of teasing interactions and its effect on social bonding. Cognition, 231: 105314. |
|
Hepach, R., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., Gerdemann, S. C., & Tomasello, M. (2023). Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation underlying helping in early childhood. Developmental Science, 26(1): e13253. |
|
Hodgetts, C. J., Close, J. O. E., & Hahn, U. (2023). Similarity and structured representation in human and nonhuman apes. Cognition, 236: 105419. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2023). Differences in the social motivations and emotions of humans and other great apes. Human Nature, 34, 588-604. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2023). Social cognition and metacognition in great apes: A theory. Animal Cognition, 26, 25-35. |
|
Vasil, J., Moore, C., & Tomasello, M. (2023). Thought and language: Association of groupmindedness with young English-speaking children’s production of pronouns. First Language, 43(5), 516-538. |
|
Wolf, W., Thielhelm, J., & Tomasello, M. (2023). Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for faces with white sclera. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 225: 105532. |
2022
Allritz, M., Call, J., Schweller, K., McEwen, E. S., de Guinea, M., Janmaat, K. R. L., Menzel, C. R., & Dolins, F. L. (2022). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) navigate to find hidden fruit in a virtual environment. Science Advances, 8: eabm4754. |
|
Call, J. (2022). The “avoid the empty cup” hypothesis does not explain great apes’ (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) responses in two three-cup one-item inference by exclusion tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 136(3), 172-188. |
|
Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2022). How fairness and dominance guide young children’s bargaining decisions. Child Development, 93(5), 1318-1333. |
|
Marno, H., Völter, C. J., Tinklenberg, B., Sperber, D., & Call, J. (2022). Learning from communication versus observation in great apes. Scientific Reports, 12: 2917. |
|
Motes-Rodrigo, A., McPherron, S. P., Archer, W., Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., & Tennie, C. (2022). Experimental investigation of orangutans’ lithic percussive and sharp stone tool behaviours. PLOS ONE, 17: e0263343. |
|
O'Madagain, C., & Tomasello, M. (2022). Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of human culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843): 20200320. |
|
O'Madagain, C., Helming, K. A., Schmidt, M. F. H., Shupe, E., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2022). Great apes and human children rationally monitor their decisions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1971): 20212686. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2022). The coordination of attention and action in great apes and humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1859): 20210093. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2022). What is it like to be a chimpanzee? Synthese, 200: 102. |
|
Vasil, J., & Tomasello, M. (2022). Effects of “we”-framing on young children’s commitment, sharing, and helping. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 214: 105278. |
|
Völter, C. J., Tinklenberg, B., Call, J., & Seed, A. M. (2022). Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 136(2), 105-120. |
2021
Allritz, M., McEwen, E. S., & Call, J. (2021). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) show subtle signs of uncertainty when choices are more difficult. Cognition, 214: 104766. |
|
Bandini, E., Großmann, J., Funk, M., Albiach‐Serrano, A., & Tennie, C. (2021). Naïve orangutans (Pongo abeliiand Pongo pygmaeus) individually acquire nut‐cracking using hammer tools. American Journal of Primatology, 83(9): e23304. |
|
Domberg, A., Tomasello, M., & Köymen, B. (2021). Collaborative reasoning in the context of group competition. PLoS One, 16: e0246589. |
|
Ebel, S. J., Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2021). Prior experience mediates the usage of food items as tools in great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo abelii). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 135(1), 64-73. |
|
Eckert, J., Rakoczy, H., Duguid, S., Herrmann, E., & Call, J. (2021). The ape lottery: Chimpanzees fail to consider spatial information when drawing statistical inferences. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 8(3), 305-324. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Zhang, Z., Zeidler, H., Dunham, Y., & Herrmann, E. (2021). The influence of friendship and merit on children’s resource allocation in three societies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 208: 105149. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Kano, F., Benziad, L., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) internal arousal remains elevated if they cannot themselves help a conspecific. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 135(2), 196-207. |
|
Howard, A., & Mundry, R. (2021). Processing geospatial data in R: A primer. In F. L. Dolins, C. A. Shaffer, L. M. Porter, J. R. Hickey, & N. P. Nibbelink ( |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Mammen, M., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Young children’s understanding of justifications for breaking a promise. Cognitive Development, 60: 101127. |
|
Li, L., Britvan, B., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Young children conform more to norms than to preferences. PLoS One, 16(5): e0251228. |
|
Mammen, M., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Young children’s moral judgments depend on the social relationship between agents. Cognitive Development, 57: 100973. |
|
Motes-Rodrigo, A., Mundry, R., Call, J., & Tennie, C. (2021). Evaluating the influence of action- and subject-specific factors on chimpanzee action copying. Royal Society Open Science, 8: 200228. |
|
Nolte, S., & Call, J. (2021). Targeted helping and cooperation in zoo-living chimpanzees and bonobos. Royal Society Open Science, 8: 201688. |
|
O'Madagain, C., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Joint attention to mental content and the social origin of reasoning. Synthese, 198(5), 4057-4078. |
|
Ordaz-Németh, I., Sop, T., Amarasekaran, B., Bachmann, M., Boesch, C., Brncic, T., Caillaud, D., Campbell, G., Carvalho, J., Chancellor, R., Davenport, T. R. B., Dowd, D., Eno-Nku, M., Ganas-Swaray, J., Granier, N., Greengrass, E., Heinicke, S., Herbinger, I., Inkamba-Nkulu, C., Iyenguet, F., Junker, J., Bobo, K. S., Lushimba, A., Maisels, F., Malanda, G. A. F., McCarthy, M. S., Motsaba, P., Moustgaard, J., Murai, M., Ndokoue, B., Nixon, S., Nseme, R. A., Nzooh, Z., Pintea, L., Plumptre, A. J., Roy, J., Rundus, A., Sanderson, J., Serckx, A., Strindberg, S., Tweh, C., Vanleeuwe, H., Vosper, A., Waltert, M., Williamson, E. A., Wilson, M., Mundry, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2021). Range-wide indicators of African great ape density distribution. American Journal of Primatology, 83(12): e23338. |
|
Plötner, M., Hepach, R., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2021). Young children share more under time pressure than after a delay. PLoS One, 16: e0248121. |
|
Romain, A., Broihanne, M.-H., De Marco, A., Ngoubangoye, B., Call, J., Rebout, N., & Dufour, V. (2021). Non-human primates use combined rules when deciding under ambiguity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1819): 20190672. |
|
Siposova, B., Grüneisen, S., Helming, K., Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2021). Common knowledge that help is needed increases helping behavior in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 201: 104973. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2021). Response to: Rethinking human development and the shared intentionality hypothesis. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 12, 465-468. |
|
Wolf, W., Nafe, A., & Tomasello, M. (2021). The development of the liking gap: Children older than 5 years think that partners evaluate them less positively than they evaluate their partners. Psychological Science, 32(5), 789-798. |
2020
Bachmann, M. E., Nielsen, M. R., Cohen, H., Haase, D., Kouassi, J. A. K., Mundry, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2020). Saving rodents, losing primates—Why we need tailored bushmeat management strategies. People and Nature, 2(4), 889-902. |
|
Bandini, E., Großmann, J., Serrano, A. A., & Tennie, C. (2020). A candidate culture-dependent form in apes, nut-cracking, can be individually learnt by naïve orangutans (Pongo abelii). bioRxiv. |
|
Boesch, C., Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., Arandjelovic, M., Pika, S., Dieguez, P., Ayimisin, A. E., Barciela, A., Coupland, C., Egbe, V. E., Eno-Nku, M., Michael Fay, J., Fine, D., Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar, R., Hermans, V., Kadam, P., Kambi, M., Llana, M., Maretti, G., Morgan, D., Murai, M., Neil, E., Nicholl, S., Ormsby, L. J., Orume, R., Pacheco, L., Piel, A., Sanz, C., Sciaky, L., Stewart, F. A., Tagg, N., Wessling, E. G., Willie, J., & Kühl, H. S. (2020). Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nature Human Behaviour. |
|
Bohn, M., Kordt, C., Braun, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Learning novel skills from iconic gestures: A developmental and evolutionary perspective. Psychological Science, 31(7), 873-880. |
|
Bräuer, J., Hanus, D., Pika, S., Gray, R. D., & Uomini, N. (2020). Old and new approaches to animal cognition: There is not “One Cognition”. Journal of Intelligence, 8(3). |
|
Bueno-Guerra, N., Colell, M., & Call, J. (2020). Effects of indirect reputation and type of rearing on food choices in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 74: 79. |
|
DeTroy, S., van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Call, J., & Haun, D. B. M. (2020). Chimp see, chimp do: The transmission of a novel skill across two groups of chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 91(3), 335-336. |
|
Duguid, S., Wyman, E., Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The strategies used by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) to solve a simple coordination problem. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 134(4), 401-411. |
|
Gopnik, A., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Introduction to special issue: ‘Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803): 20190489. |
|
Grüneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The development of coordination via joint expectations for shared benefits. Developmental Psychology, 56(6), 1149-1156. |
|
Grüneisen, S., & Wyman, E. (2020). The ontogeny and evolution of cooperation. In The Cambridge handbook of evolutionary perspectives on human behavior (pp. 265-275). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
|
Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Young children show positive emotions when seeing someone get the help they deserve. Cognitive Development, 56: 100935. |
|
Hepach, R., Benziad, L., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Chimpanzees help others with what they want; children help them with what they need. Developmental Science, 23(3): e12922. |
|
Hicks, T. C., Kühl, H. S., Boesch, C., Menken, S. B. J., Hart, J., Roessingh, P., Ewango, C., & Mundry, R. (2020). The relationship between tool use and prey availability in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Northern Democratic Republic of Congo. International Journal of Primatology, 41, 936-959. |
|
Janmaat, K., Ban, S. D., & Mundry, R. (2020). Estimating travel distance and linearity of primate routes: Ideas on how to clean and smooth track data collected with a handheld GPS. In F. L. Dolins, C. A. Shaffer, L. M. Porter, J. R. Hickey, & N. P. Nibbelink ( |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Rossano, F., Frickel, R., Tomm, A., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Children, but not great apes, respect ownership. Developmental Science, 23(1): e12842. |
|
Koomen, R. M., Grüneisen, S., & Herrmann, E. (2020). Children delay gratification for cooperative ends. Psychological Science, 31(2), 139-148. |
|
Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The early ontogeny of reason giving. Child Development Perspectives, 14(4), 215-220. |
|
Koymen, B., Jurkat, S., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Preschoolers refer to direct and indirect evidence in their collaborative reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 193: 104806. |
|
Köymen, B., O’Madagain, C., Domberg, A., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Young children’s ability to produce valid and relevant counter-arguments. Child Development, 91(3), 685-693. |
|
Lester, J. D., Vigilant, L., Gratton, P., McCarthy, M., Barratt, C. D., Boesch, C., Kühl, H. S., Dieguez, P., Aebischer, T., Agbor, A., Varona, P. Á., Angedakin, S., Assumang, A. K., Aubertf, F., Ayimisin, A. E., Bailey, E., Barubiyo, D., Bessone, M., Bonnet, M., Brazzola, G., Chancellor, R., Cipoletta, C., Cohen, H., Corogenes, K., Coupland, C., Curran, B., Danquah, E., Deschner, T., Dierks, K., Dilambaka, E., Dowd, D., Dunn, A., Dupain, J., Egbe, V. E., Goedmakers, A., Granjon, A.-C., Hedwig, D., Hermans, V., Imong, I., Jeffery, K. J., Jones, S., Junker, J., Kadam, P., Kaiser, M., Kalan, A. K., Kambere, M., Kambere, M., Kienast, I., Kujirakwinja, D., Langergraber, K. E., Lapeyre, V., Lapuente, J., Larson, B., Lee, K., Leinert, V., Llana, M., Manasseh, E.-N., Maretti, G., Marrocoli, S., Martín, R., Meier, A., Morgan, D., Mulindahabi, F., Murai, M., Neil, E., Nicholl, S., Nixon, S., Niyigaba, P., Normand, E., Orbell, C., Ormsby, L. J., Pacheco, L., Piel, A., Preece, J., Regnaut, S., Robbins, A. M., Sanz, C., Sciaky, L., Sommer, V., Stewart, F., Tagg, N., Telfer, P., Ton, E., van Schijndel, J., Vanleeuwe, H., Vergnes, V., Vyalengerera, M. K., Wessling, E. G., Willie, J., Wittig, R. M., Yuh, Y. G., Yurkiw, K., Zuberbühler, K., & Arandjelovic, M. (2020). Chimpanzee biogeography inferred from genetic diversity and effective migration. Folia Primatologica, 91(3), 264-265. |
|
Li, J., Hou, W., Zhu, L., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The development of intent-based moral judgment and moral behavior in the context of indirect reciprocity: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44(6), 525-533. |
|
Musgrave, S., Lonsdorf, E., Morgan, D., Prestipino, M., Bernstein-Kurtycz, L., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. (2020). Teaching varies with task complexity in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(2), 969-976. |
|
Pika, S., Sima, M. J., Blum, C. R., Herrmann, E., & Mundry, R. (2020). Ravens parallel great apes in physical and social cognitive skills. Scientific Reports, 10: 20617. |
|
Pouscoulous, N., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Early birds: Metaphor understanding in 3-year-olds. Journal of Pragmatics, 156, 160-167. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Do 7-year-old children understand social leverage? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 199: 104963. |
|
Schmelz, M., Grüneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2020). The psychological mechanisms underlying reciprocal prosociality in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 134(2), 149-157. |
|
Tauzin, T., Bohn, M., Gergely, G., & Call, J. (2020). Context-sensitive adjustment of pointing in great apes. Scientific Reports, 10: 1048. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2020). Kooperation und Kommunikation im zweiten Lebensjahr. In R. Braches-Chyrek, C. Röhner, H. Sünker, & M. Hopf ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2020). The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803): 20190493. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2020). The many faces of obligation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43: e89. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2020). The role of roles in uniquely human cognition and sociality. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 50(1), 2-19. |
|
Ulber, J., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Young children’s prosocial responses toward peers and adults in two social contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 198: 104888. |
|
Voinov, P. V., Call, J., Knoblich, G., Oshkina, M., & Allritz, M. (2020). Chimpanzee coordination and potential communication in a two-touchscreen turn-taking game. Scientific Reports, 10(1): 3400. |
|
Wolf, W., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Human children, but not great apes, become socially closer by sharing an experience in common ground. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 199: 104930. |
|
Wolf, W., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Watching a video together creates social closeness between children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 189: 104712. |
2019
Amici, F., Kulik, L., Langos, D., & Widdig, A. (2019). Growing into adulthood – a review on sex differences in the development of sociality across macaques. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(2): 18. |
|
Amici, F., Sánchez Amaro, A., Sebastián-Enesco, C., Allritz, M., Cachhione, T., Salazar-Bonet, J., & Rossano, F. (2019). The word order of languages predicts native speakers’ working memory. Scientific Reports, 9: 1124. |
|
Barelli, C., Gonzalez-Astudillo, V., Mundry, R., Rovero, F., Hauffe, H. C., & Gillespie, T. R. (2019). Altitude and human disturbance are associated with helminth diversity in an endangered primate, Procolobus gordonorum. PLoS One, 14(12): e0225142. |
|
Ben-Mocha, Y., Mundry, R., & Pika, S. (2019). Joint attention skills in wild Arabian babblers (Turdoides squamiceps): A consequence of cooperative breeding? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1900): 20190147. |
|
Boesch, C., Bombjaková, D., Meier, A., & Mundry, R. (2019). Learning curves and teaching when acquiring nut-cracking in humans and chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 9: 1515. |
|
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Natural reference: A phylo- and ontogenetic perspective on the comprehension of iconic gestures and vocalizations. Developmental Science, 22(2): e12757. |
|
Bohn, M., Kachel, G., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Young children spontaneously recreate core properties of language in a new modality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(51), 26072-26077. |
|
Broihanne, M.-H., Romain, A., Call, J., Thierry, B., Wascher, C. A. F., De Marco, A., Verrier, D., & Dufour, V. (2019). Monkeys (Sapajus apella and Macaca tonkeana) and great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes) play for the highest bid. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 133(3), 301-312. |
|
Bueno-Guerra, N., Völter, C. J., de las Heras, Á., Colell, M., & Call, J. (2019). Bargaining in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effect of cost, amount of gift, reciprocity, and communication. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 133(4), 542-550. |
|
Domberg, A., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children choose to reason with partners who submit to reason. Cognitive Development, 52: 100824. |
|
Ebel, S. J. (2019). Tool use in great apes and human children: The impact of prior experience and visual feedback. PhD Thesis, University of St Andrews, St Andrews. |
|
Ebel, S. J., Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2019). How prior experience and task presentation modulate innovation in 6-year-old-children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 180, 87-103. |
|
Ebel, S. J., Schmelz, M., Herrmann, E., & Call, J. (2019). Innovative problem solving in great apes: The role of visual feedback in the floating peanut task. Animal Cognition, 22(5), 791-805. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children’s sense of fairness as equal respect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(6), 454-463. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Respect defended. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(9), 716-717. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Haux, L. M., & Herrmann, E. (2019). Helping in young children and chimpanzees shows partiality towards friends. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(3), 292-300. |
|
Estienne, V. L., Robira, B., Mundry, R., Deschner, T., & Boesch, C. (2019). Acquisition of a complex extractive technique by the immature chimpanzees of Loango National Park, Gabon. Animal Behaviour, 147, 61-76. |
|
Gomes, C. M., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2019). Why do the chimpanzees of the Taï Forest share meat? The value of bartering, begging, and hunting. In C. Boesch, & R. M. Wittig ( |
|
Grocke, P., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Preschoolers consider (absent) others when choosing a distribution procedure. PLoS One, 14(8): e0221186. |
|
Grüneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children use rules to coordinate in a social dilemma. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 362-374. |
|
Hardecker, S., Buryn-Weitzel, J. C., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Adult instruction limits children’s flexibility in moral decision making. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 187: 104652. |
|
Heinicke, S., Mundry, R., Boesch, C., Amarasekaran, B., Barrie, A., Brncic, T., Brugière, D., Campbell, G., Carvalho, J., Danquah, E., Dowd, D., Eshuis, H., Fleury-Brugière, M.-C., Gamys, J., Ganas, J., Gatti, S., Ginn, L., Goedmakers, A., Granier, N., Herbinger, I., Hillers, A., Jones, S., Junker, J., Kouakou, C. Y., Lapeyre, V., Leinert, V., Marrocoli, S., Molokwu-Odozi, M., N'Goran, P. K., Normand, E., Pacheco, L., Regnaut, S., Sop, T., Ton, E., van Schijndel, J., Vendras, E., Vergnes, V., Welsh, A., Wessling, E. G., & Kühl, H. S. (2019). Characteristics of positive deviants in western chimpanzee populations. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 7: 16. |
|
Hepach, R., & Herrmann, E. (2019). The development of prosocial attention across two cultures. Frontiers in Psychology, 10: 138. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Müller, K., & Tomasello, M. (2019). The relation between young children's physiological arousal and their motivation to help others. Neuropsychologia, 126, 113-119. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Müller, K., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Toddlers’ intrinsic motivation to return help to their benefactor. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 188: 104658. |
|
Herrmann, E., Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children engage in competitive altruism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 176-189. |
|
Herrmann, E., Haux, L. M., Zeidler, H., & Engelmann, J. M. (2019). Human children but not chimpanzees make irrational decisions driven by social comparison. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: B, Biological Sciences, 286(1894): 20182228. |
|
Hicks, C., Kühl, H. S., Boesch, C., Dieguez, P., Ayimisin, A. E., Fernandez, R. M., Zungawa, D. B., Kambere, M., Swinkels, J., Menken, S. B. J., Hart, J., Mundry, R., & Roessingh, P. (2019). Bili-Uéré: A Chimpanzee Behavioural Realm in Northern Democratic Republic of Congo. Folia Primatologica, 90(1), 3-64. |
|
Horschler, D. J., Hare, B., Call, J., Kaminski, J., Miklósi, Á., & MacLean, E. L. (2019). Absolute brain size predicts dog breed differences in executive function. Animal Cognition, 22(2), 187-198. |
|
Isella, M., Kanngiesser, P., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children's selective trust in promises. Child Development, 90(6), e868-e887. |
|
Kablan, Y. A., Diarrassouba, A., Mundry, R., Campbell, G., Normand, E., Kühl, H. S., Koné, I., & Boesch, C. (2019). Effects of anti-poaching patrols on the distribution of large mammals in Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire. Oryx, 53(3), 469-478. |
|
Kachel, U., & Tomasello, M. (2019). 3- and 5-year-old children's adherence to explicit and implicit joint commitments. Developmental Psychology, 55(1), 80-88. |
|
Kachel, U., Svetlova, M., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Three- and 5-year-old children’s understanding of how to dissolve a joint commitment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 184, 34-47. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Rossano, F., Zeidler, H., Haun, D., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children's respect for ownership across diverse societies. Developmental Psychology, 55(11), 2286-2298. |
|
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2019). Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent’s action in a false-belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(42), 20904-20909. |
|
Knofe, H., Engelmann, J., Tomasello, M., & Herrmann, E. (2019). Chimpanzees monopolize and children take turns in a limited resource problem. Scientific Reports, 9: 7597. |
|
Laumer, I. B., Auersperg, A. M. I., Bugnyar, T., & Call, J. (2019). Orangutans (Pongo abelii) make flexible decisions relative to reward quality and tool functionality in a multi-dimensional tool-use task. PLoS One, 14(2): e0211031. |
|
Luncz, L. V., Mundry, R., Soiret, S., & Boesch, C. (2019). Cultural diversity of nut-cracking behaviour between two populations of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the Côte d'Ivoire. In C. Boesch, & R. M. Wittig ( |
|
Mammen, M., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children’s reasoning with peers and parents about moral dilemmas. Developmental Psychology, 55(11), 2324-2335. |
|
ManyPrimates, Altschul, D. M., Beran, M. J., Bohn, M., Call, J., DeTroy, S., Duguid, S. J., Egelkamp, C. L., Fichtel, C., Fischer, J., Flessert, M., Hanus, D., Haun, D. B. M., Haux, L. M., Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Herrmann, E., Hopper, L. M., Joly, M., Kano, F., Keupp, S., Melis, A. P., Motes Rodrigo, A., Ross, S. R., Sánchez Amaro, A., Sato, Y., Schmitt, V., Schweinfurth, M. K., Seed, A. M., Taylor, D., Völter, C. J., Warren, E., & Watzek, J. (2019). Establishing an infrastructure for collaboration in primate cognition research. PLoS One, 14(10): e0223675. |
|
Martin, R., Rochefort, J., Mundry, R., & Segelbacher, G. (2019). Delimitation of call types of Red Crossbill (Loxia curvirostra) in the Western Palearctic. Écoscience, 26(2), 177-194. |
|
Mundry, R. (2019). Developments in statistical methods applied over four decades of research, Taï Chimpanzee Project. In C. Boesch, & R. M. Wittig ( |
|
O`Madagain, C. (2019). Is reasoning culturally transmitted? Teorema: Revista internacional de filosofía, 38(1), 107-120. |
|
O`Madagain, C., Kachel, G., & Strickland, B. (2019). The origin of pointing: Evidence for the touch hypothesis. Science Advances, 5(7): eaav2558. |
|
Oller, D. K., Griebel, U., Iyer, S. N., Jhang, Y., Warlaumont, A. S., Dale, R., & Call, J. (2019). Language origins viewed in spontaneous and interactive vocal rates of human and Bonobo infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 10: 729. |
|
Proft, M., Dieball, A., & Rakoczy, H. (2019). What is the cognitive basis of the side-effect effect? An experimental test of competing theories. Mind & Language, 34(3), 357-375. |
|
Rapp, D. J., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Young children's reputational strategies in a peer group context. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 329-336. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., Altınok, N., Heintz, C., & Call, J. (2019). Disentangling great apes' decoy-effect bias in a food choice task. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 213-222. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Chimpanzees and children avoid mutual defection in a social dilemma. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(1), 46-54. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Eighteen-month old infants correct non-conforming actions by others. Infancy, 24(4), 613-635. |
|
Siposova, B., & Carpenter, M. (2019). A new look at joint attention and common knowledge. Cognition, 189, 260-274. |
|
Tennie, C., Völter, C. J., Vonau, V., Hanus, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Chimpanzees use observed temporal directionality to learn novel causal relations. Primates, 60, 517-524. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2019). The moral psychology of obligation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43: e56. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2019). Thirty years of great ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 461-469. |
|
Völter, C. J., Mundry, R., Call, J., & Seed, A. M. (2019). Chimpanzees flexibly update working memory contents and show susceptibility to distraction in the self-ordered search task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1907): 20190715. |
|
Wolf, W., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Visually attending to a video together facilitates great ape social closeness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286(1907): 20190488. |
|
Zhang, Z., Grocke, P., & Tomasello, M. (2019). The influence of intention and outcome on young children’s reciprocal sharing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 187: 104645. |
2018
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., García, L., Colmenares, F., Aboitiz, F., & Call, J. (2018). Imitation of novel conspecific and human speech sounds in the killer whale (Orcinus orca). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1871): 20172171. |
|
Amici, F., Call, J., Watzek, J., Brosnan, S., & Aureli, F. (2018). Social inhibition and behavioural flexibility when the context changes: A comparison across six primate species. Scientific Reports, 8: 3067. |
|
Ben Mocha, Y., Mundry, R., & Pika, S. (2018). Why hide? Concealed sex in dominant Arabian babblers (Turdoides squamiceps) in the wild. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 575-582. |
|
Bohn, M., & Köymen, B. (2018). Common ground and development. Child Development Perspectives, 12(2), 104-108. |
|
Bohn, M., Zimmermann, L., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The social-cognitive basis of infants' reference to absent entities. Cognition, 177, 41-48. |
|
Bueno-Guerra, N., & Amici, F. ( |
|
Domberg, A., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Children's reasoning with peers in cooperative and competitive contexts. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 64-77. |
|
Ebel, S. J., & Call, J. (2018). The interplay of prior experience and motivation in great ape problem-solving (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo abelii). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132(3), 294-305. |
|
Eckert, J., Call, J., Hermes, J., Herrmann, E., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Intuitive statistical inferences in chimpanzees and humans follow Weber’s law. Cognition, 180, 99-107. |
|
Eckert, J., Rakoczy, H., Call, J., Herrmann, E., & Hanus, D. (2018). Chimpanzees consider humans' psychological states when drawing statistical inferences. Current Biology, 28(12): e3, pp. 1959-1963. |
|
Endesfelder Quick, A., Lieven, E., Backus, E., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Constructively combining languages: The use of code-mixing in German-English bilingual child language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(3), 393-409. |
|
Endesfelder Quick, A., Lieven, E., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Identifying partially schematic units in the code-mixing of an English and German speaking child. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(4), 477-501. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Rapp, D. (2018). The influence of reputational concerns on children's prosociality. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 92-95. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Prosociality and morality in children and chimpanzees. In New perspectives on moral development (pp. 15-31). New York, NY: Routledge. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The middle step: Joint intentionality as human-unique form of second-personal engagement. In M. Jankovic, & K. Ludwig ( |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Concern for group reputation increases prosociality in young children. Psychological Science, 29(2), 181-190. |
|
Evans, C. L., Laland, K. N., Carpenter, M., & Kendal, R. L. (2018). Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly “optimal- ” rather than “over- ” imitators. Developmental Science, 21(5): e12637. |
|
Fruth, B., Tagg, N., & Stewart, F. (2018). Sleep and nesting behavior in primates: A review. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 166(3), 499-509. |
|
Gogarten, J. F., Davies, T. J., Benjamino, J., Gogarten, J. P., Graf, J., Mielke, A., Mundry, R., Nelson, M. C., Wittig, R. M., Leendertz, F. H., & Calvignac-Spencer, S. (2018). Factors influencing bacterial microbiome composition in a wild non-human primate community in Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. The ISME Journal, 12, 2559-2574. |
|
Grocke, P., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Young children are more willing to accept group decisions in which they have had a voice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 67-78. |
|
Halina, M., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The goal of ape pointing. PLoS One, 13(4): e0195182. |
|
Henkel, S., & Setchell, J. M. (2018). Group and kin recognition via olfactory cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1889): 20181527. |
|
Kachel, G., Moore, R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Two-year-olds use adults’ but not peers’ points. Developmental Science, 21(5): e12660. |
|
Kachel, U., Svetlova, M., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Three-year-olds’ reactions to a partner's failure to perform her role in a joint commitment. Child Development, 89(5), 1691-1703. |
|
Kano, F., Moore, R., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2018). Human ostensive signals do not enhance gaze following in chimpanzees, but do enhance object-oriented attention. Animal Cognition, 21(5), 715-728. |
|
Kano, F., Shepherd, S. V., Hirata, S., & Call, J. (2018). Primate social attention: Species differences and effects of individual experience in humans, great apes, and macaques. PLoS One, 13(2): e0193283. |
|
Key, F. M., Abdul-Aziz, M. A., Mundry, R., Peter, B. M., Sekar, A., D'Amato, M., Dennis, M. Y., Schmidt, J. M., & Andrés, A. M. (2018). Human local adaptation of the TRPM8 cold receptor along a latitudinal cline. PLoS Genetics, 14(5): e1007298. |
|
Koomen, R. M., & Herrmann, E. (2018). Chimpanzees overcome the tragedy of the commons with dominance. Scientific Reports, 8: 10389. |
|
Koomen, R., & Herrmann, E. (2018). An investigation of children’s strategies for overcoming the tragedy of the commons. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(5), 348-355. |
|
Koomen, R., & Herrmann, E. (2018). The effects of social context and food abundance on chimpanzee feeding competition. American Journal of Primatology, 80(2): e22734. |
|
Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Children’s meta-talk in their collaborative decision making with peers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 549-566. |
|
Krupenye, C., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos prefer individuals that hinder others over those that help. Current Biology, 28(2), 280-286.e5. |
|
Krupenye, C., Tan, J., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos voluntarily hand food to others but not toys or tools. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1886): 20181536. |
|
Li, J., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The development of intention-based sociomoral judgment and distribution behavior from a third-party stance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 78-92. |
|
Lubrich, O., Stodulka, T., & Liebal, K. (2018). Affekte im Feld – Ein blinder Fleck der Forschung? In G. Hartung, & M. Herrgen ( |
|
Luncz, L. V., Sirianni, G., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2018). Costly culture: Differences in nut-cracking efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups. Animal Behaviour, 137, 63-73. |
|
Mammen, M., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The reasons young children give to peers when explaining their judgments of moral and conventional rules. Developmental Psychology, 54(2), 254-262. |
|
McCarthy, M., Despres-Einspenner, M.-L., Samuni, L., Mundry, R., Lemoine, S., Preis, A., Wittig, R. M., Boesch, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2018). An assessment of the efficacy of camera traps for studying demographic composition and variation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 80(9): e22904. |
|
Melis, A. P., Engelmann, J. M., & Warneken, F. (2018). Chimpanzee helping is real, not a byproduct. Nature Communications, 9: 615. |
|
Misch, A., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2018). The whistleblower's dilemma in young children: When loyalty trumps other moral concerns. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 250. |
|
Morgan, D., Mundry, R., Sanz, C., Ayina, C. E., Strindberg, S., Lonsdorf, E., & Kühl, H. S. (2018). African apes coexisting with logging: Comparing chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) and gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) resource needs and responses to forestry activities. Biological Conservation, 218, 277-286. |
|
Placì, S., Eckert, J., Rakoczy, H., & Fischer, J. (2018). Long-tailed macaques (Maraca fascicularis) can use simple heuristics but fail at drawing statistical inferences from populations to samples. Royal Society Open Science, 5(9): 181025. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Chimpanzees’ understanding of social leverage. PLoS One, 13(12): e0207868. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and children in a prisoner's dilemma. Folia Primatologica, 89, 186. |
|
Schmidtke-Bode, K., & Kachel, G. (2018). Exploring the motivational antecedents of Nepalese learners of L2 English. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching. |
|
Seiler, N., Boesch, C., Stephens, C. R., Ortmann, S., Mundry, R., & Robbins, M. M. (2018). Social and ecological correlates of space use patterns in Bwindi mountain gorillas. American Journal of Primatology, 80(4): e22754. |
|
Siposova, B., Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2018). Communicative eye contact signals a commitment to cooperate for young children. Cognition, 179, 192-201. |
|
Sirianni, G., Wittig, R. M., Gratton, P., Mundry, R., Schüler, A., & Boesch, C. (2018). Do chimpanzees anticipate an object's weight? A field experiment on the kinematics of hammer-lifting movements in the nut-cracking Tai chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 21(1), 109-118. |
|
Stengelin, R., Grüneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2018). Why should I trust you? Investigating young children’s spontaneous mistrust in potential deceivers. Cognitive Development, 48, 146-154. |
|
Tagg, N., McCarthy, M., Dieguez, P., Bocksberger, G., Willie, J., Mundry, R., Stewart, F., Arandjelovic, M., Widness, J., Landsmann, A., Agbor, A., Angedakin, S., Ayimisin, A. E., Bessone, M., Brazzola, G., Corogenes, K., Heegde, M. t., Deschner, T., Dilambaka, E., Eno-Nku, M., Eshuis, H., Goedmakers, A., Granjon, A.-C., Head, J. S., Hermans, V., Jones, S., Kadam, P., Kambi, M., Langergraber, K., Lapeyre, V., Lapuente, J., Lee, K., Leinert, V., Maretti, G., Marrocoli, S., Meier, A., Nicholl, S., Normand, E., Ormsby, L. J., Piel, A., Robinson, O., Sommer, V., Tickle, A., Ton, E., van Schijndel, J., Vanleeuwe, H., Vergnes, V., Wessling, E. G., Wittig, R. M., Zuberbuehler, K., Kühl, H. S., & Boesch, C. (2018). Nocturnal activity in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence for flexible sleeping patterns and insights into human evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 166(3), 510-529. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2018). Great Apes and Human Development: A Personal History. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 189-193. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491-8498. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2018). Precis of a natural history of human morality. Philosophical Psychology, 31(5), 661-668. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2018). Response to commentators. Philosophical Psychology, 31(5), 817-829. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2018). The origins of human morality: How we learned to put our fate in one another’s hands. Scientific American, 319, 70-75. |
|
Vaish, A., Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The specificity of reciprocity: Young children reciprocate more generously to those who intentionally benefit them. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 336-353. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cohen, E., Collier-Baker, E., Rapold, C. J., Schäfer, M., Schütte, S., & Haun, D. B. M. (2018). The development of human social learning across seven societies. Nature Communications, 9: 2076. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., & Haun, D. B. M. (2018). Population-specific social dynamics in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(45), 11393-11400. |
|
Voigt, M., Wich, S. A., Ancrenaz, M., Meijaard, E., Abram, N., Banes, G., Campbell-Smith, G., d’Arcy, L. J., Delgado, R. A., Erman, A., Gaveau, D., Goossens, B., Heinicke, S., Houghton, M., Husson, S. J., Leiman, A., Sanchez, K. L., Makinuddin, N., Marshall, A. J., Meididit, A., Miettinen, J., Mundry, R., Musnanda, Nardiyono, Nurcahyo, A., Odom, K., Panda, A., Prasetyo, D., Priadjati, A., Purnomo, Rafiastanto, A., Russon, A. E., Santika, T., Sihite, J., Spehar, S., Struebig, M., Sulbaran-Romero, E., Tjiu, A., Wells, J., Wilson, K. A., & Kühl, H. S. (2018). Global demand for natural resources eliminated more than 100,000 Bornean orangutans. Current Biology, 28(5): e5, pp. 761-769. |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2018). Intuitive optics: What great apes infer from mirrors and shadows. Animal Cognition, 21(4), 493-512. |
|
Weiß, B. M., Kücklich, M., Thomsen, R., Henkel, S., Jänig, S., Kulik, L., Birkemeyer, C., & Widdig, A. (2018). Chemical composition of axillary odorants reflects social and individual attributes in rhesus macaques. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72(4): 65. |
|
Wessling, E. G., Deschner, T., Mundry, R., Pruetz, J. D., Wittig, R. M., & Kühl, H. S. (2018). Seasonal variation in physiology challenges the notion of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) as a forest-adapted species. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 6: 60. |
|
Wessling, E. G., Kühl, H. S., Mundry, R., Deschner, T., & Pruetz, J. D. (2018). The costs of living at the edge: Seasonal stress in wild savanna-dwelling chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 121, 1-11. |
|
Wu, D., Löhrich, T., Sachse, A., Mundry, R., Wittig, R. M., Calvignac‑Spencer, S., Deschner, T., & Leendertz, F. H. (2018). Seasonal and inter‑annual variation of malaria parasite detection in wild chimpanzees. Malaria Journal, 17: 38. |
2017
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Esteban, J.-A., Colmenares, F., Aboitiz, F., & Call, J. (2017). Contextual imitation of intransitive body actions in a Beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas): A "do as other does" study. PLoS One, 12(6): e0178906. |
|
Amici, F., Call, J., & Aureli, F. (2017). Coexistence of general intelligence and specialized modules. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40: e196. |
|
Amici, F., Mimó, M. C., Borell, C. v., & Bueno-Guerra, N. (2017). Meerkats (Suricata suricatta) fail to prosocially donate food in an experimental set-up. Animal Cognition, 20(6), 1059-1066. |
|
Beaune, D., Hohmann, G., Serckx, A., Sakamaki, T., Narat, V., & Fruth, B. (2017). How bonobo communities deal with fruits containing high tannin content: Re-ingestion and other feeding processes. Behavioural Processes, 142, 131-137. |
|
Bender, A., & Hanus, D. (2017). Die sozio-kulturelle Matrix menschlicher Wahrnehmung. In G. Hartung ( |
|
Benozio, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2017). Parochial compliance: Young children's biased consideration of authorities' preferences regarding intergroup interactions. Child Development, 88(5), 1527-1535. |
|
Boesch, L., Mundry, R., Kühl, H. S., & Berger, R. (2017). Wild mammals as economic goods and implications for their conservation. Ecology and Society, 22(4): 36. |
|
Bohn, M. (2017). Information and the evolution of human communication. In W. Hofkirchner ( |
|
Bohn, M., & Stöber, G. (2017). Soziale Determinanten menschlicher Kommunikation. In Max Planck Society ( |
|
Bohn, M., Allritz, M., Call, J., & Völter, C. J. (2017). Information seeking about tool properties in great apes. Scientific Reports, 7: 10923. |
|
Brandt, S., Nitschke, S., & Kidd, E. (2017). Priming the comprehension of German object relative clauses. Language Learning and Development, 13(3), 241-261. |
|
Bueno-Guerra, N. (2017). [Book review] Speechless: My recovery from stroke by Jennifer Gordon; Nedlands, WA: University Western Australia Press, 2011, 170 pages, ISBN-13: 978-0-646-55682-6. Frontiers in Psychology, 8: 1369. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS One, 12(4): e0173793. |
|
Call, J. ( |
|
Call, J. (2017). Bonobos, chimpanzees and tools: Integrating species-specific psychological biases and socio-ecology. In B. Hare, & S. Yamamoto ( |
|
Eckert, J., Rakoczy, H., & Call, J. (2017). Are great apes able to reason from multi-item samples to populations of food items? American Journal of Primatology, 79(10): e22693. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Zeller, C. (2017). Doing the right thing for the wrong reason: Reputation and moral behavior. In J. Kiverstein ( |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Clift, J. B., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Social disappointment explains chimpanzees' behaviour in the inequity aversion task. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1861): 20171502. |
|
Estienne, V., Mundry, R., Kühl, H. S., & Boesch, C. (2017). Exploitation of underground bee nests by three sympatric consumers in Loango National Park, Gabon. Biotropica, 79(1), 101-109. |
|
Feldblum, J. T., Wroblewski, E. E., Rudicell, R. S., Li, Y., Hahn, B. H., Krupenye, C., Pusey, A. E., & Gilby, I. C. (2017). Dual rank attainment strategies by male chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 162(S64), 178-178. |
|
Gonzales-Cabrera, I. (2017). On social tolerance and the evolution of human normative guidance. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 70(2), 523-549. |
|
Gretscher, H., Tempelmann, S., Haun, D. B. M., Liebal, K., & Kaminski, J. (2017). Prelinguistic human infants and great apes show different communicative strategies in a triadic request situation. PLoS One, 12(4): e0175227. |
|
Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children coordinate in a recurrent social dilemma by taking turns and along dominance asymmetries. Developmental Psychology, 53(2), 265-273. |
|
Grüneisen, S., Duguid, S., Saur, H., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of their actions for cooperators and competitors. Scientific Reports, 7: 8504. |
|
Hardecker, S., & Tomasello, M. (2017). From imitation to implementation: How two- and three-year-old children learn to enforce social norms. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 237-248. |
|
Hardecker, S., Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children's developing understanding of the conventionality of rules. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(2), 163-188. |
|
Hartmann, D., Davila-Ross, M., Wong, S. T., Call, J., & Scheumann, M. (2017). Spatial transposition tasks in Indian sloth bears (Melursus ursinus) and Bornean sun bears (Helarctos malayanus euryspilus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(4), 290-303. |
|
Haux, L. M., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Do young children preferentially trust gossip or firsthand observation in choosing a collaborative partner? Social Development, 26(3), 466-474. |
|
Hepach, R. (2017). Prosocial arousal in children. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 50-55. |
|
Hepach, R., Haberl, K., Lambert, S., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Toddlers help anonymously. Infancy, 22(1), 130-142. |
|
Hepach, R., Kante, N., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Toddlers help a peer. Child Development, 88(5), 1642-1652. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children's intrinsic motivation to provide help themselves after having accidentally harmed others. Child Development, 88(4), 1251-1264. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2017). The fulfillment of others’ needs elevates children’s body posture. Developmental Psychology, 53(1), 100-113. |
|
Hoffmann, C., Zimmermann, F., Biek, R., Kuehl, H., Nowak, K., Mundry, R., Agbor, A., Angedakin, S., Arandjelovic, M., Blankenburg, A., Brazolla, G., Corogenes, K., Couacy-Hymann, E., Deschner, T., Dieguez, P., Dierks, K., Düx, A., Dupke, S., Eshuis, H., Formenty, P., Ginath Yuh, Y., Goedmakers, A., Gogarten, J. F., Granjon, A.-C., McGraw, S., Grunow, R., Hart, J., Jones, S., Junker, J., Kiang, J., Langergraber, K., Lapuente, J., Lee, K., Leendertz, S. A., Léguillon, F., Leinert, V., Löhrich, T., Marrocoli, S., Mätz-Rensing, K., Meier, A., Merkel, K., Metzger, S., Murai, M., Niedorf, S., De Nys, H. M., Sachse, A., van Schijndel, J., Thiesen, U., Ton, E., Wu, D., Wieler, L. H., Boesch, C., Klee, S. R., Wittig, R. M., Calvignac-Spencer, S., & Leendertz, F. H. (2017). Persistent anthrax as a major driver of wildlife mortality in a tropical rainforest. Nature, 548(7665), 82-86. |
|
Jensen, K., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Punishment. In Reference Module in Life Sciences. s.l.: Elsevier. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Young children mostly keep, and expect others to keep, their promises. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 140-158. |
|
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2017). Great ape social attention. In S. Watanabe, & M. A. Hofman ( |
|
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., & Call, J. (2017). Eye tracking uncovered great apes' ability to anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(2): e1299836. |
|
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Submentalizing cannot explain belief-based action anticipation in apes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 633-634. |
|
Koomen, R. M. (2017). Exploring the evolutionary & ontogenetic roots of human strategies for overcoming the tragedy of the commons. PhD Thesis, Universität, Leipzig. |
|
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes' performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(4): e1343771. |
|
Krupenye, C., MacLean, E., & Hare, B. (2017). Does the bonobo have a (chimpanzee-like) theory of mind? In Bonobos: Unique in mind, brain and behavior (pp. 81-94). Oxford [u.a.]: Oxford Univ. Pr. |
|
Kühl, H. S., Sop, T., Williamson, E. A., Mundry, R., Brugière, D., Campbell, G., Cohen, H., Danquah, E., Ginn, L., Herbinger, I., Jones, S., Junker, J., Kormos, R., Kouakou, C. Y., N'Goran, P. K., Normand, E., Shutt-Phillips, K., Tickle, A., Vendras, E., Welsh, A., Wessling, E. G., & Boesch, C. (2017). The critically endangered western chimpanzee declines by 80%. American Journal of Primatology, 79(9): e22681. |
|
Leeuwen, E. J. C. v., & Call, J. (2017). Conservatism and “copy-if-better” in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 20(3), 575-579. |
|
Lewis, A. V. M., Call, J., & Berntsen, D. (2017). Distinctiveness enhances long-term event memory in non-human primates, irrespective of reinforcement. American Journal of Primatology, 79(8): e22665. |
|
Lewis, A. V. M., Call, J., & Berntsen, D. (2017). Non-goal-directed recall of specific events in apes after long delays. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 284(1858): 20170518. |
|
Liebal, K., & Rossano, F. (2017). The give and take of food sharing in Sumatran orang-utans, Pongo abelii, and chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 133, 91-100. |
|
MacLean, E. L., Herrmann, E., Suchindran, S., & Hare, B. (2017). Individual differences in cooperative communicative skills are more similar between dogs and humans than chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 126, 41-51. |
|
Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2017). Individual differences in children's corepresentation of self and other in joint action. Child Development, 88(3), 964-978. |
|
Mundry, R. (2017). From nonparametric tests to mixed models: A brief overview of statistical tools frequently used in comparative psychology. In J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall ( |
|
Rapp, D. J., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2017). The impact of choice on young children's prosocial motivation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 158, 112-121. |
|
Ruiz-Lambides, A., Weiß, B. M., Kulik, L., Stephens, C., Mundry, R., & Widdig, A. (2017). Long-term analysis on the variance of extra-group paternities in rhesus macaques. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 71(4): 67. |
|
Samuni, L., Preis, A., Mundry, R., Deschner, T., Crockford, C. ǂ., & Wittig, R. M. (2017). Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(2), 268-273. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A. (2017). Coordination in conflict situations: A comparative investigation of the coordination strategies that children, chimpanzees and bonobos use to solve situations of conflict. PhD Thesis, Universität, Leipzig. |
|
Sanchez-Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Chimpanzees, bonobos and children successfully coordinate in conflict situations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1856): 20170259. |
|
Schaebs, F. S., Perry, S. E., Cohen, D., Mundry, R., & Deschner, T. (2017). Social and demographic correlates of male androgen levels in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). American Journal of Primatology, 79(7): e22653. |
|
Schmelz, M. *., Grueneisen, S. *., Kabalak, A., Jost, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Chimpanzees return favors at a personal cost. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(28), 7462-7467. |
|
Schmelz, M., Duguid, S., Bohn, M., & Völter, C. J. (2017). Cooperative problem solving in giant otters (Pteronura brasiliensis) and Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea). Animal Cognition, 20(6), 1107-1114. |
|
Schmid, B., Karg, K., Perner, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes are sensitive to prior reliability of an informant in a gaze following task. PLoS One, 12(11): e0187451. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Gonzalez-Cabrera, I., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children’s developing metaethical judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 163-177. |
|
Schneider, C., Liebal, K., & Call, J. (2017). “Giving” and “responding” differences in gestural communication between nonhuman great ape mothers and infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(3), 303-313. |
|
Seiler, N., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., Stephens, C. R., & Robbins, M. M. (2017). Space partitioning in wild, non-territorial mountain gorillas: The impact of food and neighbours. Royal Society Open Science, 4(11), 1-13. |
|
Surbeck, M., Girard-Buttoz, C., Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Fruth, B., Hohmann, G., Langergraber, K., Zuberbühler, K., Wittig, R. M., & Mundry, R. (2017). Sex-specific association patterns in bonobos and chimpanzees reflect species differences in cooperation. Royal Society Open Science, 4: 161081. |
|
Svetlova, M., & Carpenter, M. (2017). Social development. In Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development (pp. 415-423). Cambridge [u.a.]: Cambridge University Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2017). What did we learn from the ape language studies? In B. Hare, & S. Yamamoto ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Gonzalez-Cabrera, I. (2017). The role of ontogeny in the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 28(3), 274-288. |
|
Ulber, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Young children, but not chimpanzees, are averse to both disadvantageous and advantageous inequities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 48-66. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Mundry, R., Cronin, K. A., Bodamer, M., & Haun, D. B. M. (2017). Chimpanzee culture extends beyond matrilineal family units. Current Biology, 27(12), R588-R590. |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2017). Causal and inferential reasoning in animals. In G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall ( |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2017). Cognition. In A. Fuentes ( |
|
Völter, C. J., Rossano, F., & Call, J. (2017). Social manipulation in nonhuman primates: Cognitive and motivational determinants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 76-94. |
|
Wessling, E. G., Kühl, H. S., Deschner, T., Mundry, R., & Pruetz, J. (2017). Seasonal variation in indicators of climatic and ecological stress in savannah-dwelling chimpanzees. Folia primatologica, 88(2), 113. |
|
White, L. C., & Austin, J. J. (2017). Relict or reintroduction? Genetic population assignment of three Tasmanian devils (Sarcophilus harrisii) recovered on mainland Australia. Royal Society Open Science, 4(4): 170053. |
|
Widdig, A., Muniz, L., Minkner, M., Barth, Y., Bley, S., Ruiz-Lambides, A., Junge, O., Mundry, R., & Kulik, L. (2017). Low incidence of inbreeding in a long-lived primate population isolated for 75 years. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 71(1): 18. |
2016
Allritz, M., Call, J., & Borkenau, P. (2016). How chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) perform in a modified emotional Stroop task. Animal Cognition, 19(3), 435-449. |
|
Bohn, M. (2016). Social cognition and human communication: Reference to absent entities in great apes and human infants. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Comprehension of iconic gestures by chimpanzees and human children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 1-17. |
|
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). The role of past interactions in great apes' communication about absent entities. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(4), 351-357. |
|
Borries, C., Sandel, A. A., Koenig, A., Fernandez-Duque, E., Kamilar, J. M., Amoroso, C. R., Barton, R. A., Bray, J., Fiore, A. D., Gilby, I. C., Gordon, A. D., Mundry, R., Port, M., Powell, L. E., Pusey, A. E., Spriggs, A., & Nunn, C. L. (2016). Transparency, usability, and reproducibility: Guiding principles for improving comparative databases using primates as examples. Evolutionary Anthropology, 25(5), 232-238. |
|
Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2016). German children's use of word order and case marking to interpret simple and complex sentences: Testing differences between constructions and lexical items. Language Learning and Development, 12(2), 156-182. |
|
Bueno-Guerra, N., Leiva, D., Colell, M., & Call, J. (2016). Do sex and age affect strategic behavior and inequity aversion in children? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 285-300. |
|
Butler, L. P., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Two- and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical cues in guiding inductive generalization and exploration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145, 64-78. |
|
Cacchione, T., Hrubesch, C., Call, J., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Are apes essentialists? Scope and limits of psychological essentialism in great apes. Animal Cognition, 19(5), 921-937. |
|
Cameron, K. N., Reed, P., Morgan, D. B., Ondzié, A. I., Sanz, C. M., Kühl, H. S., Olson, S. H., Leroy, E., Karesh, W. B., & Mundry, R. (2016). Spatial and temporal dynamics of a mortality event among Central African great apes. PLoS One, 11(5): e0154505. |
|
Christie, S., Gentner, D., Call, J., & Haun, D. B. M. (2016). Sensitivity to relational similarity and object similarity in apes and children. Current Biology, 26(4), 531-535. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., & Herrmann, E. (2016). Chimpanzees trust their friends. Current Biology, 26(2), 252-256. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Preschoolers affect others' reputations through prosocial gossip. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 447-460. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2016). The effects of being watched on resource acquisition in chimpanzees and human children. Animal Cognition, 19(1), 147-151. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., Rapp, D., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children (sometimes) do the right thing even when their peers do not. Cognitive Development, 39, 86-92. |
|
Goldstone, L. G., Sommer, V., Nurmi, N., Stephens, C. R., & Fruth, B. (2016). Food begging and sharing in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus): Assessing relationship quality? Primates, 57(3), 367-376. |
|
Grocke, P. (2016). Procedural justice in children: Preschooler's use of and reaction to fair and unfair decision-making procedures. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Hanus, D. (2016). Causal reasoning versus associative learning: A useful dichotomy or a strawman battle in comparative psychology? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(3), 241-248. |
|
Hardecker, S., Schmidt, M. F. H., Roden, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children’s behavioral and emotional responses to different social norm violations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 364-379. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). How to compare across species. Psychological Science, 27(12), 1670-1672. |
|
Hepach, R. (2016). Motive for young children's developing concern for others' well-Being as a core motive for developing prosocial behavior. In C. Brand ( |
|
Hepach, R., & Westermann, G. (2016). Pupillometry in infancy research. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(3), 359-377. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children want to see others get the help they need. Child Development, 87(6), 1703-1714. |
|
Hood, B., Weltzien, S., Marsh, L., & Kanngiesser, P. (2016). Picture yourself: Self-focus and the endowment effect in preschool children. Cognition, 152, 70-77. |
|
Ibbotson, P., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Evidence rebuts Chomsky's theory of language learning. Scientific American, 315(5). Retrieved from https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/evidence-rebuts-chomsky-s-theory-of-language-learning/. |
|
Imong, I. S., Kühl, H. S., Robbins, M. M., & Mundry, R. (2016). Evaluating the potential effectiveness of alternative management scenarios in ape habitat. Environmental Conservation, 43(2), 161-171. |
|
John, M. (2016). The origins of value attribution in children (Homo sapiens sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Kalan, A., Piel, A. K., Mundry, R., Wittig, R. M., Boesch, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2016). Passive acoustic monitoring reveals group ranging and territory use: A case study of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Frontiers in Zoology, 13: 34. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., & Woike, J. K. (2016). Framing the debate on human-like framing effects in bonobos and chimpanzees: A comment on Krupenye et al. (2015). Biology Letters, 12(1): 20150718. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Schmidt, M. F. H., & Rossano, F. (2016). Young children's understanding of social norms and social institutions. In H. Kury ( |
|
Kano, F., Hirata, S., Deschner, T., Behringer, V., & Call, J. (2016). Nasal temperature drop in response to a playback of conspecific fights in chimpanzees: A thermo-imaging study. Physiology & Behavior, 155, 83-94. |
|
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2 perspective-taking? Animal Cognition, 19(3), 555-564. |
|
Ko, A.-M.-S. (2016). Early Austronesians and genetic prehistory of Taiwan. PhD Thesis, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig. |
|
Köymen, B., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Çocuklar sosyal normları nasıl algılar? In Ç. Aydın, T. Göksun, A. C. Küntay, & D. Tahiroğlu ( |
|
Köymen, B., Lieven, E., & Brandt, S. (2016). Syntactic and semantic coordination in finite complement-clause constructions: A diary-based case study. Journal of Child Language, 43(1), 22-42. |
|
Köymen, B., Mammen, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Preschoolers use common ground in their justificatory reasoning with peers. Developmental Psychology, 52(3), 423-429. |
|
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110-114. |
|
Martínez-Patiño, M. J., Vilain, E., & Bueno-Guerra, N. (2016). The unfinished race: 30 years of gender verification in sport. The Lancet, 388(10044), 541-543. |
|
Melis, A. P., Grocke, P., Kalbitz, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). One for you, one for me: Humans' unique turn-taking skills. Psychological Science, 27(7), 987-996. |
|
Misch, A., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2016). I won't tell: Young children show loyalty to their group by keeping group secrets. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 96-106. |
|
Mundry, R., & Oelze, V. M. (2016). Who is who matters - the effects of pseudoreplication in stable isotope analysis. American Journal of Primatology, 78(10), 1017-1030. |
|
Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. (2016). Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 6: 34783. |
|
Over, H., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Do young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members? Cognitive Development, 40, 24-32. |
|
Pfefferle, D., Hammerschmidt, K., Mundry, R., Ruiz-Lambides, A., Fischer, J., & Widdig, A. (2016). Does the structure of female rhesus macaque coo calls reflect relatedness and/or familiarity? PLoS One, 11(8): e0161133. |
|
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). What is a group? Young children’s perceptions of different types of groups and group entitativity. PLoS One, 11(3): e0152001. |
|
Riedl, K. (2016). Third-party punishment in chimpanzees and children. PhD Thesis, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig. |
|
Sanchez-Amaro, A., & Amici, F. (2016). Markets carefully interpreted: A reply to Kaburu and Newton-Fisher (2016). Animal Behaviour, 119, e7-e13. |
|
Sanchez-Amaro, A., Duguid, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Chimpanzees coordinate in a snowdrift game. Animal Behaviour, 116, 61-74. |
|
Sanchez-Amaro, A., Pereto, M., & Call, J. (2016). Differences in between-reinforcer value modulate the selective-value effect in great apes (Pan Troglodyes, P. Paniscus, Gorilla Gorilla, Pongo Abelii). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(1), 1-12. |
|
Schmelz, M., & Call, J. (2016). The psychology of primate cooperation and competition: A call for realigning research agendas. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686): 20150067. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2016). How chimpanzees cooperate: If dominance is artificially constrained. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(44), E6728-E6729. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Butler, L. P., Heinz, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children see a single action and infer a social norm: Promiscuous normativity in 3-year-olds. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1360-1370. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Hardecker, S., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Preschoolers understand the normativity of cooperatively structured competition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 34-47. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., Mietzsch, T., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children understand the role of agreement in establishing arbitrary norms – but unanimity is key. Child Development, 87(2), 612-626. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Svetlova, M., Johe, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally. Cognitive Development, 37, 42-52. |
|
Silk, J. B., & House, B. R. (2016). The evolution of altruistic social preferences in human groups. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 371(1687): 20150097. |
|
Song, R., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2016). Young children discriminate genuine from fake smiles and expect people displaying genuine smiles to be more prosocial. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(6), 490-501. |
|
Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., Premo, L. S., & McPherron, S. P. (2016). The island test for cumulative culture in the paleolithic. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus ( |
|
Tennie, C., Jensen, K., & Call, J. (2016). The nature of prosociality in chimpanzees. Nature Communications, 7: 13915. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2016). A natural history of human morality. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univ. Pr. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2016). Eine Naturgeschichte der menschlichen Moral. Berlin: Suhrkamp. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2016). Cultural learning redux. Child Development, 87(3), 643-653. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2016). In Memoriam: Jerome Seymour Bruner [1915–2016]. Cognition, 155, iii-iv. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2016). The ontogeny of cultural learning. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 1-4. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Ibbotson, P. (2016). Language in a new key. Scientific American, 315, 70-75. |
|
Ulber, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Extrinsic rewards diminish costly sharing in 3-year-olds. Child Development, 87(4), 1192-1203. |
|
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). The early emergence of guilt-motivated prosocial behavior. Child Development, 87(6), 1772-1782. |
|
Vaish, A., Herrmann, E., Markmann, C., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Preschoolers value those who sanction non-cooperators. Cognition, 153, 43-51. |
|
Vogelsang, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Giving is nicer than taking: Preschoolers reciprocate based on the social intentions of the distributor. PLoS One, 11(1): e0147539. |
|
Völter, C. J., Sentís, I., & Call, J. (2016). Great apes and children infer causal relations from patterns of variation and covariation. Cognition, 155, 30-43. |
|
Zeidler, H., Herrmann, E., Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Taking turns or not? Children’s approach to limited resource problems in three different cultures. Child Development, 87(3), 677-688. |
2015
Albiach-Serrano, A., Sebastián-Enesco, C., Seed, A., Colmenares, F., & Call, J. (2015). Comparing humans and nonhuman great apes in the broken cloth problem: Is their knowledge causal or perceptual? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 174-189. |
|
Ambridge, B., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2015). A constructivist account of child language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney ( |
|
Amici, F. (2015). The evolution and development of human cooperation. Interaction studies, 16(3), 383-418. |
|
Amici, F., & Bietti, L. (2015). Coordination, collaboration and cooperation: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Interaction studies, 16(3), vii-xii. |
|
Amici, F., Call, J., Brosnan, S., Watzek, J., & Aureli, F. (2015). When the social context changes: Social inhibition and fission-fusion dynamics. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 238-239. |
|
Barney, B. J., Amici, F., Aureli, F., Call, J., & Johnson, V. E. (2015). Joint bayesian modeling of binomial and rank data for primate cognition. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 110(510), 573-582. |
|
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Communication about absent entities in great apes and human infants. Cognition, 145, 63-72. |
|
Bräuer, J., & Call, J. (2015). Apes produce tools for future use. American Journal of Primatology, 77(3), 254-263. |
|
Brncic, T., Amarasekaran, B., McKenna, A., Mundry, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2015). Large mammal diversity and their conservation in the human-dominated land-use mosaic of Sierra Leone. Biodiversity and Conservation, 24(10), 2417-2438. |
|
Butler, L. P., Schmidt, M. F. H., Bürgel, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Young children use pedagogical cues to modulate the strength of normative inferences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 476-488. |
|
Cacchione, T., & Amici, F. (2015). Cohesion as a principle for perceiving objecthood: Does it apply to animate agents? Swiss Journal of Psychology, 74(4), 217-228. |
|
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The relationship between infant holdout and gives, and pointing. Infancy, 20(5), 576-586. |
|
Carducci, P., Trapanese, C., Hanus, D., & Truppa, V. (2015). Does the Presentation Format Affect Learning Speed in Visual Discrimination Tasks? A Study on Tufted Capuchin Monkeys ( Sapajus spp.). Folia primatologica, 86(4), 257. |
|
Close, J., & Call, J. (2015). From colour photographs to black-and-white line drawings: An assessment of chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes') transfer behaviour. Animal Cognition, 18(2), 437-449. |
|
Damerius, L., Forss, S., Kosonen, Z., Burkart, J., Haun, D., Liebal, K., Call, J., Galdikas, B., & van Schaik, C. (2015). Determinants of variation in orangutans' cognitive performance: I. effects of background and housing conditions. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 266. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Chimpanzees trust conspecifics to engage in low-cost reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1801): 20142803. |
|
Forss, S., Damerius, L., Kosonen, Z., Burkart, J., Call, J., Haun, D., Liebal, K., Galdikas, B., & van Schaik, C. (2015). Determinants of variation in orangutans' cognitive performance: II. influences of rearing conditions and human exposure. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 282. |
|
Gomez-Puerto, G., Munar, E., Kano, F., & Call, J. (2015). Eye-tracking of primate's preference for curvature. Perception, 44 (S1), 32. |
|
Graf, E., Theakston, A., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Subject and object omission in children's early transitive constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(3), 701-727. |
|
Grassmann, S., Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1200. |
|
Grimm, A. *., Weiß, B. M. *., Kulik, L., Mihoub, J.-B., Mundry, R., Köppen, U., Brueckmann, T., Thomsen, R., & Widdig, A. (2015). Earlier breeding, lower success: does the spatial scale of climatic conditions matter in a migratory passerine bird? Ecology and Evolution, 5(23), 5722-5734. |
|
Grocke, P., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 197-210. |
|
Gross, R. L., Drummond, J., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2015). Individual differences in toddlers’ social understanding and prosocial behavior: Disposition or socialization? Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 600. |
|
Grosse, K., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Differences in the ability of apes and children to instruct others using gestures. Language Learning and Development, 11(4), 310-330. |
|
Grüneisen, S. (2015). The development of strategic coordination: An investigation into children's abilities to solve coordination problems. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Grüneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). "I know you don't know I know..": Children use second-order false belief reasoning for peer coordination. Child Development, 86(1), 287-293. |
|
Grüneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Children use salience to solve coordination problems. Developmental Science, 18(3), 495-501. |
|
Grüneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Conforming to coordinate: Children use majority information for peer coordination. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 136-147. |
|
Grünloh, T., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Young children's intonational marking of new, given and contrastive referents. Language Learning and Development, 11(2), 95-127. |
|
Haidle, M. N., Bolus, M., Collard, M., Conard, N. J., Garofoli, D., Lombard, M., Nowell, A., Tennie, C., & Whiten, A. (2015). The nature of culture: An eight-grade model for the evolution and expansion of cultural capacities in hominins and other animals. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 93, 43-70. |
|
Hanus, D., Truppa, V., & Call, J. (2015). Visual illusions in great apes, monkeys and humans. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 290. |
|
Haun, D. B. M. (2015). Comparative and developmental anthropology: Studying the origins of cultural variability in cognitive function. In L. A. Jensen ( |
|
Hedwig, D., Mundry, R., Robbins, M. M., & Boesch, C. (2015). Audience effects, but not environmental influences, explain variation in gorilla close distance vocalizations. A test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. American Journal of Primatology, 77(12), 1239-1252. |
|
Hedwig, D., Mundry, R., Robbins, M. M., & Boesch, C. (2015). Contextual correlates of syntactic variation in mountain and western gorilla close-distance vocalizations: Indications for lexical or phonological syntax? Animal Cognition, 18(2), 423-435. |
|
Henkel, S., Ruiz-Lambides, A., Berger, A., Thomsen, R., & Widdig, A. (2015). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) recognize group membership via olfactory cues alone. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69(12), 2019-2034. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Novel paradigms to measure variability of behavior in early childhood: Posture, gaze, and pupil dilation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 858. |
|
Herrmann, E. (2015). Origins of human cognition and temperament: A systematic comparison of non-human great apes and human children. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 295-296. |
|
Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Focusing and shifting attention in human children and chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(3), 268-274. |
|
Herrmann, E., Misch, A., Hernandez-Lloreda, V., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Uniquely human self-control begins at school age. Developmental Science, 18(6), 979-993. |
|
John, M., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Do chimpanzees distinguish between collaborators and non-collaborators after food acquisition? Folia primatologica, 86(4), 299-300. |
|
John, M., Rossano, F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Scarcity and value attribution in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Folia primatologica, 86(4), 300. |
|
Junge, B., Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2015). Given-new/new-given? Children's sensitivity to the ordering of information in complex sentences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(3), 589-612. |
|
Junker, J., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2015). Integrating wildlife conservation with conflicting economic land-use goals in a West African biodiversity hotspot. Basic and Applied Ecology, 16(8), 690-702. |
|
Junker, J., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., Stephens, C. R., Lormie, M., Tweh, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2015). Education and access to fish but not economic development predict chimpanzee and mammal occurrence in West Africa. Biological Conservation, 182, 27-35. |
|
Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2015). Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Animal Behaviour, 101, 1-9. |
|
Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., Wagner, O. J. J., Heinicke, S., Boesch, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2015). Towards the automated detection and occupancy estimation of primates using passive acoustic monitoring. Ecological Indicators, 54, 217-226. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Itakura, S., Zhou, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Hood, B. (2015). The role of social eye-gaze in children’s and adults’ ownership attributions to robotic agents in three cultures. Interaction studies, 16(1), 1-28. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Late emergence of the first possession heuristic: Evidence from a small-scale culture. Child Development, 86(4), 1282-1289. |
|
Kano, F., Hirata, S., & Call, J. (2015). Social attention in the two species of pan: Bonobos make more eye contact than chimpanzees. PLoS One, 10(6): e0129684. |
|
Karg, K. (2015). Clashing perspectives: Mentalizing in great apes. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can see. Animal Cognition, 18(5), 1069-1076. |
|
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience to infer what a competitor can see? Animal Behaviour, 105, 211-221. |
|
Koomen, R., & Herrmann, E. (2015). Feast or famine: An investigation of scarcity and co-feeding effects in chimpanzees. Folia primatologica, 86(4), 306-307. |
|
Köymen, B., Schmidt, M. F. H., Rost, L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Teaching versus enforcing game rules in preschoolers' peer interactions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 135, 93-101. |
|
Kulik, L., Amici, F., Langos, D., & Widdig, A. (2015). Sex differences in the development of aggressive behavior in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). International Journal of Primatology, 36(4), 764-789. |
|
Kulik, L., Amici, F., Langos, D., & Widdig, A. (2015). Sex differences in the development of social relationships in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). International Journal of Primatology, 36(2), 353-376. |
|
Kupczik, K., Stark, H., Mundry, R., Neininger, F. T., Heidlauf, T., & Röhrle, O. (2015). Reconstruction of muscle fascicle architecture from iodine-enhanced microCT images: A combined texture mapping and streamline approach. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 382, 34-43. |
|
Le Guen, O., Samland, J., Friedrich, T., Hanus, D., & Brown, P. (2015). Making sense of (exceptional) causal relations. A cross-cultural and cross-linguistic study. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 1645. |
|
Marín Manrique, H., & Call, J. (2015). Age-dependent cognitive inflexibility in great apes. Animal Behaviour, 102, 1-6. |
|
Masi, S., Mundry, R., Ortmann, S., Cipolletta, C., Boitani, L., & Robbins, M. M. (2015). The influence of seasonal frugivory on nutrient and energy intake in wild western gorillas. PLoS One, 10(7): e0129254. |
|
Melis, A. P., Floedl, A., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Non-egalitarian allocations among preschool peers in a face-to-face bargaining task. PLoS One, 10(3): e0120494. |
|
Misch, A. (2015). Young children's loyalty to the group. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Moore, R., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Production and comprehension of gestures between orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus) in a referential communication game. PLoS One, 10(6): e0129726. |
|
Moore, R., Müller, B., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Developmental Science, 18(2), 232-242. |
|
Munar, E., Gómez-Puerto, G., Call, J., & Nadal, M. (2015). Common visual preference for curved contours in humans and great apes. PLoS One, 10(11): e0141106. |
|
Nichols, S. R., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2015). Toddlers' responses to infants' negative emotions. Infancy, 20(1), 70-97. |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2015). Children infer affiliative and status relations from watching others imitate. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 917-925. |
|
Petit, O., Dufour, V., Herrenschmidt, M., Marco, A. D., Sterck, E. H. M., & Call, J. (2015). Inferences about food location in three cercopithecine species: An insight into the socioecological cognition of primates. Animal Cognition, 18(4), 821-830. |
|
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on children’s prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 161-173. |
|
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Young children show the bystander effect in helping situations. Psychological Science, 26(4), 499-506. |
|
Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Restorative justice in children. Current Biology, 25(13), 1731-1735. |
|
Roca, E. M., Gomez-Puerto, G., Nadal, M., & Call, J. (2015). Do great apes also prefer curved visual objects? Perception, 44 (S1), 64-65. |
|
Rossano, F., Fiedler, L., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Preschoolers' understanding of the role of communication and cooperation in establishing property rights. Developmental Psychology, 51(2), 176-184. |
|
Sánchez Amaro, A., & Amici, F. (2015). Are primates out of the market? Animal Behaviour, 110, 51-60. |
|
Schaefer, M., Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Fair is not fair everywhere. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1252-1260. |
|
Schmelz, M., Krüger, O., Call, J., & Krause, E. T. (2015). A comparison of spontaneous problem-solving abilities in three estrildid Finch (Taeniopygia guttata, Lonchura striata var. domestica, Stagonopleura guttata) species. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(4), 356-365. |
|
Schmerse, D., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Young children use shared experience to interpret definite reference. Journal of Child Language, 42(5), 1146-1157. |
|
Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). 18-month-olds comprehend indirect communicative acts. Cognition, 136, 91-98. |
|
Seifart, F., & Mundry, R. (2015). Quantitative comparative linguistics based on tiny corpora: N-gram language identification of wordlists of known and unknown languages from Amazonia and beyond. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 22(3), 202-214. |
|
Sirianni, G., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2015). When to choose which tool: Multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 100, 152-165. |
|
Song, R. (2015). The importance of belonging and affiliation in development. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Song, R., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2015). Children draw more affiliative pictures following priming with third-party ostracism. Developmental Psychology, 51(6), 831-840. |
|
Spiegel, I., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Evolutionäre Anthropologie: Kooperation im Wir-Modus. In J. Nida-Rümelin ( |
|
Stewart, L., MacLean, E. L., Ivy, D., Woods, V., Cohen, E., Rodriguez, K., McIntyre, M., Mukherjee, S., Call, J., Kaminski, J., Miklósi, Á., Wrangham, R. W., & Hare, B. (2015). Citizen science as a new tool in dog cognition research. PLoS One, 10(9): e0135176. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Ibbotson, P., Freudenthal, D., Lieven, E. V., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Productivity of noun slots in verb frames. Cognitive Science, 39(6), 1369-1395. |
|
Truppa, V., Carducci, P., Trapanese, C., & Hanus, D. (2015). Does presentation format influence visual size discrimination in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.)? PLoS One, 10(4): e0126001. |
|
Ulber, J. (2015). The developmental origins of fairness: Distributive justice in early childhood. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Ulber, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2015). How 18- and 24-month-old peers divide resources among themselves. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 228-244. |
|
Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2015). Person-centred positive emotions, object-centred negative emotions: 2-year-olds generalize negative but not positive emotions across individuals. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 391-397. |
|
Völter, C. J., Rossano, F., & Call, J. (2015). From exploitation to cooperation: Social tool use in orang-utan mother–offspring dyads. Animal Behaviour, 100, 126-134. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The developmental and evolutionary origins of human helping and sharing. In D. A. Schroeder, & W. G. Graziano ( |
|
Wobber, V., & Herrmann, E. (2015). The influence of testosterone on cognitive performance in bonobos and chimpanzees. In B. Hare, & S. Yamamoto ( |
|
Wobber, V., & Herrmann, E. (2015). The influence of testosterone on cognitive performance in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour, 152(3-4), 407-423. |
|
Zeidler, H. (2015). Crosscultural aspects of social cognition in children. PhD Thesis, Universität, Leipzig. |
2014
Albiach-Serrano, A., & Call, J. (2014). A reversed-reward contingency task reveals causal knowledge in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 17(5), 1167-1176. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) help conspecifics to obtain food? Folia Primatologica, 85(1), 45-46. |
|
Amici, F. (2014). How new world monkeys see the social world. In H. A. Barrera Saldaña ( |
|
Amici, F., & Bräuer, J. (2014). Monkey physical cognition: A comparative review. In H. A. Barrera Saldaña ( |
|
Amici, F., Aureli, F., & Call, J. (2014). Response facilitation in the four great apes: Is there a role for empathy? Primates, 55(1), 113-118. |
|
Amici, F., Aureli, F., Mundry, R., Sanchez-Amaro, A., Barroso, A. M., Ferretti, J., & Call, J. (2014). Calculated reciprocity? A comparative test with six primate species. Primates, 55(3), 447-457. |
|
Amici, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J. (2014). Lack of prosociality in great apes, capuchin monkeys and spider monkeys: Convergent evidence from two different food distribution tasks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1793): 20141699. |
|
Austin, K., Theakston, A., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children’s understanding of denial. Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2061-2070. |
|
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children create iconic gestures to inform others. Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2049-2060. |
|
Beier, J. S., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2014). Young children help others to achieve their social goals. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 934-940. |
|
Bourjade, M., Call, J., Pelé, M., Maumy, M., & Dufour, V. (2014). Bonobos and orangutans, but not chimpanzees, flexibly plan for the future in a token-exchange task. Animal Cognition, 17(6), 1329-1340. |
|
Bozek, K., Wei, Y., Yan, Z., Liu, X., Xiong, J., Sugimoto, M., Tomita, M., Pääbo, S., Pieszek, R., Sherwood, C. C., Hof, P. R., Ely, J. J., Steinhauser, D., Bangsbo, J., Willmitzer, L., Hansson, O., Call, J., Giavalisco, P., & Khaitovich, P. (2014). Exceptional evolutionary divergence of human muscle and brain metabolomes parallels human cognitive and physical uniqueness. PLoS Biology, 12(5): e1001871. |
|
Bräuer, J. (2014). Klüger als wir denken: Wozu Tiere fähig sind. Berlin [u.a.]: Springer Spektrum. |
|
Bräuer, J. (2014). What dogs understand about humans. In J. Kaminski ( |
|
Bullinger, A. F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) instrumentally help but do not communicate in a mutualistic cooperative task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128(3), 251-260. |
|
Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., Huber, J., Isler, K., Kosonen, Z. K., Martins, E., Meulman, E., Richiger, R., Rueth, K., Spillmann, B., Wiesendanger, S., & van Schaik, C. P. (2014). The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications, 5: 4747. |
|
Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2014). Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition, 130(1), 116-127. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 120-126. |
|
Cacchione, T., Hrubesch, C., & Call, J. (2014). Apes' tracking of objects and collections. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 73(1), 47-52. |
|
Cacchione, T., Hrubesch, C., & Call, J. (2014). Phylogenetic roots of quantity processing: Apes do not rely on object indexing to process quantities. Cognitive Development, 31, 79-95. |
|
Cacchione, T., Indino, M., Fujita, K., Itakura, S., Matsuno, T., Schaub, S., & Amici, F. (2014). Universal ontology: Attentive tracking of objects and substances across languages and over development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(6), 481-486. |
|
Call, J. (2014). Why do apes cooperate? Folia Primatologica, 85(1), 43-43. |
|
Carpenter, M. (2014). Imitation (role of) in communicative development. In P. J. Brooks, & V. Kempe ( |
|
Cohen, E., Mundry, R., & Kirschner, S. (2014). Religion, synchrony and cooperation. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 4(1), 20-30. |
|
Cronin, K. A., Pieper, B., Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Mundry, R., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Problem solving in the presence of others: How rank and relationship quality impact resource acquisition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). PLoS One, 9(4): e93204. |
|
Cronin, K. A., Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Vreeman, V., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Population-level variability in the social climates of four chimpanzee societies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(5), 389-396. |
|
Dahlin, C. R., Young, A. M., Cordier, B., Mundry, R., & Wright, T. F. (2014). A test of multiple hypotheses for the function of call sharing in female budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68(1), 145-161. |
|
Darusman, H. S., Call, J., Sajuthi, D., Schapiro, S. J., Gjedde, A., Kalliokoski, O., & Hau, J. (2014). Delayed response task performance as a function of age in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Primates, 55(2), 259-267. |
|
Darusman, H. S., Pandelaki, J., Mulyadi, R., Sajuthi, D., Putri, I. A., Kalliokoski, O. H., Call, J., Abelson, K. S., Schapiro, S. J., Gjedde, A., & Hau, J. (2014). Poor memory performance in aged cynomolgus monkeys with hippocampal atrophy, depletion of amyloid beta 1-42 and accumulation of tau proteins in cerebrospinal fluid. In Vivo, 28(2), 173-184. |
|
De Nys, H. M., Calvignac-Spencer, S., Boesch, C., Dorny, P., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Leendertz, F. H. (2014). Malaria parasite detection increases during pregnancy in wild chimpanzees. Malaria Journal, 13: 413. |
|
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Familiar verbs are not always easier than novel verbs: How german pre-school children comprehend active and passive sentences. Cognitive Science, 38(1), 128-151. |
|
Duguid, S. (2014). Coordination for cooperation: A comparative study of the coordination strategies used by chimpanzees and human children. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Duguid, S., Wyman, E., Bullinger, A. F., Herfurth-Majstorovic, K., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in a Stag Hunt game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1796): 20141973. |
|
Engelmann, J. M. (2014). An empirical investigation of the evolutionary and ontogenetic roots of trust. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Fruth, B., Bondjengo Ikombe, N., Kitengie Matshimba, G., Metzger, S., Musuyu Muganza, D., Mundry, R., & Fowler, A. (2014). New evidence for self-medication in bonobos: Manniophyton fulvum leaf- and stemstrip-swallowing from LuiKotale, Salonga National Park, DR Congo. American Journal of Primatology, 76(2), 146-158. |
|
Funwi-Gabga, N., Kühl, H. S., Maisels, F. G., Cheyne, S. M., Wich, S. A., & Williamson, E. (2014). The status of apes across Africa and Asia. In Extractive Industries and Ape Conservation (pp. 252-277). Cambridge [u.a.]: Cambridge Univ. Pr. |
|
Garamszegi, L. Z., & Mundry, R. (2014). Multimodel-inference in comparative analyses. In L. Z. Garamszegi ( |
|
Göckeritz, S. (2014). Children constructing a social world: Exploring preschoolers' understanding of social norms. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Göckeritz, S., Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children's creation and transmission of social norms. Cognitive Development, 30, 81-95. |
|
Hamann, K., Bender, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Meritocratic sharing is based on collaboration in 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 121-128. |
|
Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2014). When maths trumps logic: Probabilistic judgements in chimpanzees. Biology Letters, 10(12): 20140892. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Children conform to the behavior of peers; Other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2160-2167. |
|
Hedwig, D., Hammerschmidt, K., Mundry, R., Robbins, M. M., & Boesch, C. (2014). Acoustic structure and variation in mountain and western gorilla close calls: a syntactic approach. Behaviour, 151(8), 1091-1120. |
|
Hertel, A., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Generalize or personalize - do dogs transfer an acquired rule to novel situations and persons? PLoS One, 9(7): 10266. |
|
Hribar, A., Sonesson, G., & Call, J. (2014). From sign to action: Studies in chimpanzee pictorial competence. Semiotica, 2014(198), 205-240. |
|
Ibbotson, P., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). The communicative contexts of grammatical aspect use in English. Journal of Child Language, 41(3), 705-723. |
|
Imong, I. S., Robbins, M. M., Mundry, R., Bergl, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2014). Distinguishing ecological constraints from human activity in species range fragmentation: The case of Cross River gorillas. Animal Conservation, 17(4), 323-331. |
|
Imong, I. S., Robbins, M. M., Mundry, R., Bergl, R., & Kühl, H. S. (2014). Informing conservation management about structural versus functional connectivity: A case-study of Cross River gorillas. American Journal of Primatology, 76(10), 978-988. |
|
Jensen, K., Vaish, A., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2014). The emergence of human prosociality: Aligning with others through feelings, concerns, and norms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 822. |
|
Kaminski, J., & Marshall-Pescini, S. ( |
|
Kanngiesser, P., & Hood, B. M. (2014). Not by labor alone: Considerations for value influence use of the labor rule in ownership transfers. Cognitive Science, 38(2), 353-366. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., & Hood, B. M. (2014). Young children's understanding of ownership rights for newly made objects. Cognitive Development, 29(1), 30-40. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Itakura, S., & Hood, B. M. (2014). The effect of labour on ownership decisions in two cultures: Developmental evidence from Japan and the United Kingdom. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 320-329. |
|
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014). Cross-species variation in gaze following and conspecific preference among great apes, human infants and adults. Animal Behaviour, 91, 137-150. |
|
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014). Great apes generate goal-based action predictions: An eye-tracking study. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1691-1698. |
|
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). All great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) and two-and-a-half-year-old children (Homo sapiens) discriminate appearance from reality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128(4), 431-439. |
|
Kinzler, K. D., & Vaish, A. (2014). Political infants? Developmental origins of the negativity bias. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 318-318. |
|
Kirschner, S., & Ilari, B. (2014). Joint drumming in Brazilian and German preschool children: Cultural differences in rhythmic entrainment, but no prosocial effects. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(1), 137-166. |
|
Köymen, B., & Kyratzis, A. (2014). Dialogic syntax and complement constructions in toddlers' peer interactions. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(3), 497-521. |
|
Köymen, B., Lieven, E. V. M., Engemann, D. A., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Children's norm enforcement in their interactions with peers. Child Development, 85(3), 1108-1122. |
|
Köymen, B., Rosenbaum, L., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Reasoning during joint decision-making by preschool peers. Cognitive Development, 32, 74-85. |
|
Köymen, B., Schmerse, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children create partner-specific referential pacts with peers. Developmental Psychology, 50(10), 2334-2342. |
|
Krajewski, G., & Lieven, E. (2014). Competing cues in early syntactic development. In B. MacWhinney, A. L. Malchukov, & E. Moravcsik ( |
|
Liebal, K. (2014). The psychology of gestures and gesture-like movements in non-human primates. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & J. Bressem ( |
|
Liebal, K., Vaish, A., Haun, D., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Does sympathy motivate prosocial behaviour in great apes? PLoS One, 9(1): e84299. |
|
Lieven, E. (2014). First language learning from a usage-based approach. In T. Herbst ( |
|
Lieven, E. V. M. (2014). First language development: A usage-based perspective on past and current research. Journal of Child Language, 41(Suppl. 1), 48-63. |
|
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., Baker, J. M., Bania, A. E., Barnard, A. M., Boogert, N. J., Brannon, E. M., Bray, E. E., Bray, J., Brent, L. J. N., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., Cantlon, J. F., Cheke, L. G., Clayton, N. S., Delgado, M. M., DiVincenti, L. J., Fujita, K., Herrmann, E., Hiramatsu, C., Jacobs, L. F., Jordan, K. E., Laude, J. R., Leimgruber, K. L., Messer, E. J. E., de Moura, A. C. A., Ostojić, L., Picard, A., Platt, M. L., Plotnik, J. M., Range, F., Reader, S. M., Reddy, R. B., Sandel, A. A., Santos, L. R., Schumann, K., Seed, A. M., Sewall, K. B., Shaw, R. C., Slocombe, K. E., Su, Y., Takimoto, A., Tan, J., Tao, R., van Schaik, C. P., Virányi, Z., Visalberghi, E., Wade, J. C., Watanabe, A., Widness, J., Young, J. K., Zentall, T. R., & Zhao, Y. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(20), E2140-E2148. |
|
Martin Ordas, G., Atance, C. M., & Call, J. (2014). Remembering in tool-use tasks in children and apes: The role of the information at encoding. Memory, 22(1), 129-144. |
|
Mayer, C., Call, J., Albiach-Serrano, A., Visalberghi, E., Sabbatini, G., & Seed, A. (2014). Abstract knowledge in the broken-string problem: Evidence from nonhuman primates and pre-schoolers. PLoS One, 9(10): e108597. |
|
Mendes, N., & Call, J. (2014). Chimpanzees form long-term memories for food locations after limited exposure. American Journal of Primatology, 76(5), 485-495. |
|
Misch, A., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2014). Stick with your group: Young children’s attitudes about group loyalty. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 19-36. |
|
Moll, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Two- and three-year-olds know what others have and have not heard. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 12-21. |
|
Moore, R. (2014). Ontogenetic constraints on Paul Grice's theory of communication. In D. Matthews ( |
|
Mundry, R. (2014). Statistical issues and assumptions of phylogenetic generalized least squares. In L. Z. Garamszegi ( |
|
Nitzschner, M. (2014). Cooperation in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Nitzschner, M., Kaminski, J., Melis, A., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Side matters: potential mechanisms underlying dogs' performance in a social eavesdropping paradigm. Animal Behaviour, 90, 263-271. |
|
Pelé, M., Broihanne, M. H., Thierry, B., Call, J., & Dufour, V. (2014). To bet or not to bet? Decision-making under risk in non-human primates. The Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 49(2), 141-166. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Clüver, A., Saucke, L., Stoffregen, N., Gräbener, A., Migura, J., & Call, J. (2014). Apes are intuitive statisticians. Cognition, 131(1), 60-68. |
|
Richter, N. (2014). The use of physical self-similarity in social assortment in children. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Rossano, F., & Liebal, K. (2014). “Requests” and “offers” in orangutans and human infants. In P. Drew ( |
|
Rossano, F., Nitzschner, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Domestic dogs and puppies can use human voice direction referentially. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1785): 20133201. |
|
Sabbatini, G., Marín Manrique, H., Trapanese, C., Vizioli, A. D. B., Call, J., & Visalberghi, E. (2014). Sequential use of rigid and pliable tools in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Animal Behaviour, 87(1), 213-220. |
|
Salas, L., Palacios-Huerta, I., & Call, J. (2014). Intertemporal preferences in great apes. Folia Primatologica, 85(1), 56-57. |
|
Samuni, L., Mundry, R., Terkel, J., Zuberbühler, K., & Hobaiter, H. (2014). Socially learned habituation to human observers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 17(4), 997-1005. |
|
Sanchez-Amaro, A., Peretó, M., Colell, M., & Call, J. (2014). Natural choices of food in chimpanzees and orangutans. Folia Primatologica, 85(1), 57-57. |
|
Schäfer, M. (2014). Cultural variation in children's development of resource sharing and fairness. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schmerse, D. (2014). Children's acquisition of grounding elements: investigations of definiteness and discourse deixis. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schmerse, D., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Discourse particles and belief reasoning: The case of German "doch". Journal of Semantics, 31(1), 115-133. |
|
Schreiweis, C., Bornschein, U., Burguière, E., Kerimoglu, C., Schreiter, S., Dannemann, M., Goyal, S., Rea, E., French, C. A., Puliyadi, R., Groszer, M., Fisher, S. E., Mundry, R., Winter, C., Hevers, W., Pääbo, S., Enard, W., & Graybiel, A. M. (2014). Humanized Foxp2 accelerates learning by enhancing transitions from declarative to procedural performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(39), 14253-14258. |
|
Schulze, C. (2014). Young children's comprehension of indirect communication: Empirical research and theoretical conclusions. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Seed, A. M., & Call, J. (2014). Space or physics? Children use physical reasoning to solve the trap problem from 2.5 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 50(7), 1951-1962. |
|
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Do domestic dogs learn words based on humans’ referential behaviour? PLoS One, 9(3): e91014. |
|
Tennie, C., Walter, V., Gampe, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Limitations to the cultural ratchet effect in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 152-160. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Harvard: Harvard Univ. Pr. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2014). Eine Naturgeschichte des menschlichen Denkens. Berlin: Suhrkamp. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2014). Introduction to the Classic edition. In M. Tomasello ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2014). Kooperation und Kommunikation im zweiten Lebensjahr. In R. Braches-Chyrek, C. Röhner, H. Sünker, & M. Hopf ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European journal of social psychology, 44(3), 187-194. |
|
Uskul, A. K., & Over, H. (2014). Responses to social exclusion in cultural context: Evidence from farming and herding communities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(5), 752-771. |
|
Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2014). The early ontogeny of human cooperation and morality. In M. Killen, & J. Smetana ( |
|
Van der Goot, M. H., Tomasello, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2014). Differences in the nonverbal requests of great apes and human infants. Child Development, 85(2), 444-455. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Call, J., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Human children rely more on social information than chimpanzees do. Biology Letters, 10(11): 20140487. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., & Haun, D. B. M. (2014). A group-specific arbitrary tradition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 17(6), 1421-1425. |
|
Vogelsang, M., Jensen, K., Kirschner, S., Tennie, C., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Preschoolers are sensitive to free riding in a public goods game. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 729. |
|
Völter, C. (2014). Problem solving in great apes: On the effect of visual feedback, causal inferences, and planning. PhD Thesis, Bielefeld Univ., Bielefeld. |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2014). Great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii) follow visual trails to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128(2), 199-208. |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2014). The cognitive underpinnings of flexible tool use in great apes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 40(3), 287-302. |
|
Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2014). Younger apes and human children plan their moves in a maze task. Cognition, 130(2), 186-203. |
|
Waller, B. M., Misch, A., Whitehouse, J., & Herrmann, E. (2014). Children, but not chimpanzees, have facial correlates of determination. Biology Letters, 10(3): 20130974. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Motivation Science, 1(S), 43-48. |
|
Warneken, F., Steinwender, J., Hamann, K., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children's planning in a collaborative problem-solving task. Cognitive Development, 31, 48-58. |
|
Wilson, M. L., Boesch, C., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Gilby, I. C., Hashimoto, C., Hobaiter, C. L., Hohmann, G., Itoh, N., Koops, K., Lloyd, J. N., Matsuzawa, T., Mitani, J. C., Mjungu, D. C., Morgan, D., Muller, M. N., Mundry, R., Nakamura, M., Pruetz, J., Pusey, A. E., Riedel, J., Sanz, C., Schel, A. M., Simmons, N., Waller, M., Watts, D. P., White, F., Wittig, R. M., Zuberbühler, K., & Wrangham, R. W. (2014). Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature, 513(7518), 414-417. |
|
Wobber, V., Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Wrangham, R., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(3), 547-573. |
2013
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, V., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2013). Experimental evidence for action imitation in killer whales (Orcinus orca). Animal Cognition, 16(1), 11-22. |
|
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, V., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2013). Relative quantity judgments in the beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas) and the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Processes, 96, 11-19. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Herrmann, E., Suzumura, T., Adachi, I., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2013). Do rank and age affect primates' inhibitory control? Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata) and Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) compared. Folia primatologica, 84(3-5), 240. |
|
Allritz, M., Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2013). Food washing and placer mining in captive great apes. Primates, 54(4), 361-370. |
|
Amici, F., & Majolo, B. (2013). How to make comparative cognition really comparative. Folia primatologica, 84, 241-242. |
|
Amici, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J. (2013). Prosocial behaviour across six primate species: The effect of different paradigms. Folia primatologica, 84, 241. |
|
Bannard, C., Klinger, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material? Developmental Psychology, 49(12), 2344-2356. |
|
Barelli, C., Mundry, R., Heistermann, M., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2013). Cues to androgens and quality in male gibbon songs. PLoS One, 8(12): 82748. |
|
Bercelli, F., Rossano, F., & Viaro, M. (2013). Supra-session courses of action in psychotherapy. Journal of Pragmatics, 57, 118-137. |
|
Blasse, A., Calvignac-Spencer, S., Merkel, K., Goffe, A. S., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., & Leendertz, F. H. (2013). Mother-offspring transmission and age-dependent accumulation of Simian foamy virus in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Virology, 87(9), 5193-5204. |
|
Bräuer, J., Bös, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 273-285. |
|
Bräuer, J., Keckeisen, M., Pitsch, A., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Domestic dogs conceal auditory but not visual information from others. Animal Cognition, 16(3), 351-359. |
|
Bräuer, J., Schönefeld, K., & Call, J. (2013). When do dogs help humans? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 148(1-2), 138-149. |
|
Brownell, C. A., & Svetlova, M. (2013). Emergence and early development of the body image. In V. Slaughter, & C. A. Brownell ( |
|
Brownell, C. A., Iesue, S. S., Nichols, S. R., & Svetlova, M. (2013). Mine or yours? Development of sharing in toddlers in relation to ownership understanding. Child Development, 84(3), 906-920. |
|
Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., Anderson, R., Nichols, S. R., & Drummond, J. (2013). Socialization of early prosocial behavior: Parents’ talk about emotions is associated with sharing and helping in toddlers. Infancy, 18(1), 91-119. |
|
Bullinger, A. F., Burkart, J. M., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Bonobos, Pan paniscus, chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, prefer to feed alone. Animal Behaviour, 85(1), 51-60. |
|
Butler, L. P., & Walton, G. M. (2013). The opportunity to collaborate increases preschoolers’ motivation for challenging tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 953-961. |
|
Buttelmann, D., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? Animal Cognition, 16(1), 137-145. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions, but fail to imitate them. Animal Behaviour, 86(4), 755-761. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(2), 422-428. |
|
Cacchione, T., Hrubesch, C., & Call, J. (2013). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) quantify split solid objects. Animal Cognition, 16(1), 1-10. |
|
Call, J. (2013). Three ingredients for becoming a creative tool user. In C. M. Sanz, J. Call, & C. Boesch ( |
|
Call, J., Amici, F., & Aureli, F. (2013). Social tolerance and inhibitory control in primates: Variation, sociality, and its relation to socio-ecology and cognition. Folia primatologica, 84, 255. |
|
Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2013). How joint is the joint attention of apes and human infants? In J. Metcalfe, & H. S. Terrace ( |
|
Carpenter, M., Uebel, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(5), 1511-1518. |
|
Cohen, E., & Haun, D. B. M. (2013). The development of tag-based cooperation via a socially acquired trait. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 230-235. |
|
Cronin, K. A., Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2013). The many metrics of social tolerance: Are we measuring one concept? Folia primatologica, 84(3-5), 261-262. |
|
Darusman, H., Sajuthi, D., Kalliokoski, O., Jacobsen, K., Call, J., Schapiro, S., Gjedde, A., Abelson, K., & Hau, J. (2013). Correlations between serum levels of beta amyloid, cerebrospinal levels of tau and phospho tau, and delayed response tasks in young and aged cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of Medical Primatology, 42(3), 137-146. |
|
Daum, M. M., Ulber, J., & Gredebäck, G. (2013). The development of pointing perception in infancy: Effects of communicative signals on covert shifts of attention. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1898-1908. |
|
De Nys, H. M., Calvignac-Spencer, S., Thiesen, U., Boesch, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Leendertz, F. H. (2013). Age-related effects on malaria parasite infection in wild chimpanzees. Biology Letters, 9(4): 20121160. |
|
Dediu, D., Cysouw, M., Levinson, S. C., Baronchelli, A., Christiansen, M. H., Croft, W., Evans, N., Garrod, S., Gray, R. D., Kandler, A., & Lieven, E. (2013). Cultural evolution of language. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Dube, A. (2013). Visual and tactile communication of a captive hamadryas baboon group (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) with special regard to their intentionality. PhD Thesis, Univ., Halle-Wittenberg. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Over, H., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and potential reciprocators. Developmental Science, 16(6), 952-958. |
|
Gräfenhain, M., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds’ understanding of the consequences of joint commitments. PLoS One, 8(9): e73039. |
|
Große, G., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds hide their communicative intentions in appropriate contexts. Developmental Psychology, 49(11), 2095-2101. |
|
Grossmann, T., Vaish, A., Franz, J., Schroeder, R., Stoneking, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2013). Emotional voice processing: Investigating the role of genetic variation in the serotonin transporter across development. PLoS One, 8(7): e68377. |
|
Grueter, C., Ndamiyabo, F., Plumtre, A. J., Abavandimwe, D., Mundry, R., Fawcett, K. A., & Robbins, M. M. (2013). Long-term temporal and spatial dynamics of food availability for endangered mountain gorillas in Volcanoes National Park, Rwanda. American Journal of Primatology, 75(3), 267-280. |
|
Halina, M., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal Cognition, 16(4), 653-666. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Over, H. (2013). Like me: A homophily-based account of human culture. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Wertenbruch, M. (2013). Forschungen und Entwicklungen zum Konzept der Ehre als Potential für Konflikte zwischen Kulturen. Wien: Österreichischer Integrationsfonds. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., & Edelson, M. G. (2013). Majority influence in children and other animals. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 61-71. |
|
Hepach, R., & Westermann, G. (2013). Infants’ sensitivity to the congruence of others’ emotions and actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 16-29. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). A New Look at Children’s Prosocial Motivation. Infancy, 18(1), 67-90. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children sympathize less in response to unjustified emotional distress. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 1132-1138. |
|
Herrmann, E., Keupp, S., Hare, B., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(1), 63-75. |
|
Ibbotson, P., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The attention-grammar interface: Eye-gaze cues structural choice in children and adults. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(3), 457-481. |
|
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzee responders still behave like rational maximizers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(20): E1837. |
|
Jordan, F. M., van Schaik, C., Francois, P., Gintis, H., Haun, D. B. M., Hruschka, D. H., Janssen, M. A., Kitts, J. A., Lehmann, L., Mathew, S., Richerson, P. J., Turchin, P., & Wiessner, P. (2013). Cultural evolution of the structure of human groups. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Kaminski, J., & Nitzschner, M. (2013). Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44(4), 294-302. |
|
Kaminski, J., Pitsch, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Dogs steal in the dark. Animal Cognition, 16(3), 385-394. |
|
Kirschner, S. (2013). The upside of sharing. MaxPlanckResearch, (4), 18-24. Retrieved from http://www.mpg.de/7644731/F001_Focus_018-025.pdf. |
|
Köymen, B., & Küntay, A. C. (2013). Turkish children's conversational oppositions: Usage of two discourse markers. Discourse Processes, 50(6), 388-406. |
|
Lallée, S., Hamann, K., Steinwender, J., Warneken, F., Martienz, U., Barron-Gonzales, H., Pattacini, U., Gori, I., Petit, M., Metta, G., Verschure, P., & Dominey, P. F. (2013). Cooperative human robot interaction systems: IV. Communication of shared plans with Naive humans using gaze and speech. In 2013 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (pp. 129-136). |
|
Langos, D., Kulik, L., Mundry, R., & Widdig, A. (2013). The impact of paternity on male–infant association in a primate with low paternity certainty. Molecular Ecology, 22(13), 3638-3651. |
|
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children's understanding of cultural common ground. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(1), 88-96. |
|
Liedtke, F., & Schulze, C. ( |
|
Lieven, E. V. M. (2013). Appendix: Developmental issues. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Lieven, E. V. M. (2013). Language acquisition as a cultural process. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Lieven, E. V. M., & Stoll, S. (2013). Early Communicative Development in Two Cultures: A Comparison of the Communicative Environments of Children from Two Cultures. Human Development, 56(3), 178-206. |
|
Luef, E. M., & Liebal, K. (2013). The hand-on gesture in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). Interaction studies, 14(1), 44-61. |
|
Manrique, H. M., Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2013). Repeated innovation in great apes. Animal Behaviour, 85(1), 195-202. |
|
Martin Ordas, G., Berntsen, D., & Call, J. (2013). Memory for distant past events in chimpanzees and orangutans. Current Biology, 23(15), 1438-1441. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., & Call, J. (2013). Episodic memory: a comparative approach. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7: 63. |
|
Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biology Letters; The Royal Society, London, [England], 9(2): 20130009. |
|
Melis, A. P., Altrichter, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Allocation of resources to collaborators and free-riders in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 364-370. |
|
Menzel, C., Fowler, A., Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2013). Leaf Surface Roughness Elicits Leaf Swallowing Behavior in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and Bonobos (P. paniscus), but not in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) or Orangutans (Pongo abelii). International Journal of Primatology, 34(3), 533-553. |
|
Mesoudi, A., Laland, K. N., Boyd, R., Buchanan, B., Flynn, E., McCauley, R. N., Renn, J., Reyes-Garcia, V., Shennan, S., Stout, D., & Tennie, C. (2013). The cultural evolution of technology and science. In P. J. Richerson, & M. H. Christiansen ( |
|
Moll, H., Meltzoff, A. N., Merzsch, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Taking Versus Confronting Visual Perspectives in Preschool Children. Developmental Psychology, 49(4), 646-654. |
|
Moore, R. (2013). Evidence and interpretation in great ape gestural communication. Humana.Mente Journal of Philosophical Studies, 24, 27-51. |
|
Moore, R. (2013). Imitation and conventional communication. Biology and Philosophy, 28(3), 481-500. |
|
Moore, R. (2013). Social learning and teaching in chimpanzees. Biology and Philosophy, 28(6), 879-901. |
|
Moore, R., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Interaction studies, 14(1), 62-80. |
|
Nichols, S. R., Brownwell, C. A., & Svetlova, M. (2013). Converging developments in prosocial behavior and self-other understanding in the second year of life: The second social-cognitive revolution. In M. R. Banaji, & S. A. Gelman ( |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2013). The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 6-11. |
|
Over, H., Carpenter, M., Spears, R., & Gattis, M. (2013). Children selectively trust individuals who have imitated them. Social Development, 22(2), 215-224. |
|
Péron, F., John, M., Sapowicz, S., Bovet, D., & Pepperberg, I. M. (2013). A study of sharing and reciprocity in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Cognition, 16(2), 197-210. |
|
Rakoczy, H., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2013). The early ontogeny of social norms. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 17-21. |
|
Rosati, A. G., Herrmann, E., Kaminski, J., Krupenye, C., Melis, A. P., Schroepfer, K., Tan, J., Warneken, F., Wobber, V., & Hare, B. (2013). Assessing the psychological health of captive and wild apes: A response to Ferdowsian et al. (2011). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(3), 329-336. |
|
Rossano, F. (2013). Sequence organization and timing of bonobo mother-infant interactions. Interaction studies, 14(2), 160-189. |
|
Salomo, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Children's ability to answer different types of questions. Journal of Child Language, 40(2), 469-491. |
|
Sanz, C. M., Call, J., & Boesch, C. ( |
|
Scheider, L., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Do domestic dogs interpret pointing as a command? Animal Cognition, 16(3), 361-372. |
|
Schmelz, M. (2013). Theory of mind in chimpanzees: New insights by means of a competitive paradigm. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees predict that a competitor’s preference will match their own. Biology Letters, 9(1): 20120829. |
|
Schmerse, D., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Error patterns in young German children's wh-questions. Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 656-671. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children understand and defend the entitlements of others. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 930-944. |
|
Schmitt, V., Kroeger, I., Zinner, D., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2013). Monkeys perform as well as apes and humans in a size discrimination task. Animal Cognition, 16(5), 829-838. |
|
Schneider, A.-C. (2013). The ability of chimpanzees to solve complex cooperative problems. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schubert, G., Vigilant, L., Boesch, C., Klenke, R., Langergraber, K. E., Mundry, R., Surbeck, M., & Hohmann, G. (2013). Co–residence between males and their mothers and grandmothers is more frequent in bonobos than chimpanzees. PLoS One, 8(12): e83870. |
|
Schulze, C. (2013). Relevance inferences in young children: 3-year-olds understand a speaker’s indirectly expressed social intention. In F. Liedtke, & C. Schulze ( |
|
Schulze, C., Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2013). 3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication. Child Development, 84(6), 2079-2093. |
|
Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M., Yun, J.-e., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18(1), 40-66. |
|
Stoll, S., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2013). Studying language acquisition cross-linguistically. In H. Winskel, & P. Padakannaya ( |
|
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Liebal, K. (2013). When apes point the finger: Three great ape species fail to use a conspecific’s imperative pointing gesture. Interaction studies, 14(1), 7-23. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2013). Gestural conventions. In Y. Milev ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Dueling dualists: Commentary on Carpendale, Atwood, and Kettner. Human Development, 56(6), 401-405. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2013). Why don't apes understand false beliefs? In M. R. Banaji, & S. A. Gelman ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual review of psychology, 64, 231-255. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Schütte, S., Call, J., & Haun, D. B. M. (2013). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) flexibly adjust their behaviour in order to maximize payoffs, not to conform to majorities. PLoS One, 8(11): e80945. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J., & Haun, D. B. M. (2013). Conformity in nonhuman primates: Fad or fact? Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 1-7. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18(3), 345-368. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 338-350. |
|
Wittig, M., Jensen, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Five-year-olds understand fair as equal in a mini-ultimatum game. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 324-337. |
|
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Non-verbal communication enables children’s coordination in a “Stag Hunt” game. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(5), 597-610. |
2012
Acerbi, A., Jacquet, P. O., & Tennie, C. (2012). Behavioral constraints and the evolution of faithful social learning. Current Zoology, 58(2), 307-318. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Bräuer, J., Cacchione, T., Zickert, N., & Amici, F. (2012). The effect of domestication and ontogeny in swine cognition (Sus scrofa scrofa and S. s. domestica). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 141(1-2), 25-35. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Bugnyar, T., & Call, J. (2012). Apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, P. troglodytes, Pongo abelii) versus corvids (Corvus corax, C. corone) in a support task: The effect of pattern and functionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(4), 355-367. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Call, J., & Barth, J. (2012). Erratum: Great apes track hidden objects after changes in the objects' position and in subject's orientation. American Journal of Primatology, 74(10), 967-. |
|
Amici, F., Barney, B., Johnson, V. E., Call, J., & Aureli, F. (2012). A modular mind? A test using individual data from seven primate species. PLoS One, 7(12): e51918. |
|
Amici, F., Call, J., & Aureli, F. (2012). Aversion to violation of expectations of food distribution: the role of social tolerance and relative dominance in seven primate species. Behaviour, 149(3-4), 345-368. |
|
Bannard, C., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Can we dissociate contingency learning from social learning in word acquisition by 24-month-olds? PLoS One, 7(11): e49881. |
|
Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds' comprehension and production of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 359-375. |
|
Bourjade, M., Thierry, B., Call, J., & Dufour, V. (2012). Are monkeys able to plan for future exchange? Animal Cognition, 15(5), 783-795. |
|
Braeuer, J., & Hanus, D. (2012). Fairness in non-human primates? Social Justice Research, 25(3), 256-276. |
|
Bräuer, J. (2012). [Book review] In Defence of Dogs: Why Dogs Need Our Understanding by J. Bradshaw (2011). Published by Penguin Books Ltd. 324 pages Hardback (ISBN 978-1-846-14295-6). Animal Welfare, 21(3), 442-443. |
|
Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2012). Finding the Cause: Verbal Framing Helps Children Extract Causal Evidence Embedded in a Complex Scene. Journal of Cognition & Development, 13(1), 38-66. |
|
Buttelmann, D. (2012). Understanding different components in intentional action: how understanding others' mental states helps to learn from others and predict their behaviour. Hamburg: Kovač. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Schütte, S., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Great apes infer others' goals based on context. Animal Cognition, 15(6), 1037-1053. |
|
Call, J. (2012). Seeking information in non-human animals: weaving a metacognitive web. In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust ( |
|
Call, J., & Hribar, A. (2012). Great apes' strategies for locating hidden objects in small-scale space. Cognitive Processing, 13(S1), S16-S17. |
|
Call, J., & Santos, L. R. (2012). Understanding Other Minds. In J. C. Mitani, & J. Call ( |
|
Carpenter, M. (2012). Joint attention in humans and animals. In N. M. Seel ( |
|
Carpenter, M., & Liebal, K. (2012). Joint Attention, Communication, and Knowing Together in Infancy. In A. Seemann ( |
|
Close, J., & Pothos, E. M. (2012). "Object Categorization: Reversals and Explanations of the Basic-Level Advantage" (Rogers & Patterson, 2007): A simplicity account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(8), 1615-1632. |
|
Cohen, E. (2012). The evolution of tag-based cooperation in humans: The case for accent. Current Anthropology, 53(5), 588-616. |
|
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22(2), 142-146. |
|
Cronin, K. A. (2012). Cognitive aspects of prosocial behavior in nonhuman primates. In N. M. Seel ( |
|
Cronin, K. A., & Sanchez, A. (2012). Social dynamics and cooperation: The case of nonhuman primates and its implications for human behavior. Advances in complex systems, 15(supp01): 1250066. |
|
Curé, C., Mathevon, N., Mundry, R., & Aubin, T. (2012). Acoustic cues used for species recognition can differ between sexes and sibling species: evidence in shearwaters. Animal Behaviour, 84(1), 239-250. |
|
Decety, J., & Svetlova, M. (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience: a Journal for Cognitive, Affective and Social Developmental Neuroscience, 2(1), 1-24. |
|
Deschner, T., Fuller, B. T., Oelze, V. M., Boesch, C., Hublin, J.-J., Mundry, R., Richards, M. P., Ortmann, S., & Hohmann, G. (2012). Identification of energy consumption and nutritional stress by isotopic and elemental analysis of urine in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Rapid Communications in Mass Spectrometry, 26(1), 69-77. |
|
Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLoS One, 7(10): e48433. |
|
Fletcher, G. E., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Differences in cognitive processes underlying the collaborative activities of children and chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 27(2), 136-153. |
|
Gampe, A., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language, 32(3), 385-397. |
|
Goffe, A. S., Blasse, A., Mundry, R., Leendertz, F. H., & Calvignac-Spencer, S. (2012). Detection of retroviral super-infection from non-invasive samples. PLoS One, 7(5): e36570. |
|
Grassmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and naming. Animal Cognition, 15(4), 657-665. |
|
Gretscher, H., Haun, D. B., Liebal, K., & Kaminski, J. (2012). Orang-utans rely on orientation cues and egocentric rules when judging others' perspectives in a competitive food task. Animal Behaviour, 84(2), 323-331. |
|
Grosse, G., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two-year-old children differentiate test questions from genuine questions. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 192-204. |
|
Hamann, K. (2012). Collaboration and the development of fairness. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Children's Developing Commitments to Joint Goals. Child Development, 83(1), 137-145. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22, 727-731. |
|
Head, J. S., Robbins, M. M., Mundry, R., Makaga, L., & Boesch, C. (2012). Remote video-camera traps measure habitat use and competitive exclusion among sympatric chimpanzee, gorilla and elephant in Loango National Park, Gabon. Journal of Tropical Ecology, 28(6), 571-583. |
|
Hepach, R. (2012). The motivation of early benevolence: An investigation into a causal mechanism of cooperation. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped. Psychological Science, 23(9), 967-972. |
|
Herrmann, E. (2012). Similarities in and differences between nonhuman ape and human cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. In N. M. Seel ( |
|
Herrmann, E., & Call, J. (2012). Are there geniuses among the apes? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2753-2761. |
|
Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Human cultural cognition. In J. C. Mitani, J. Call, & P. M. Kappeler ( |
|
Hesler, N., Mundry, R., & Dabelsteen, T. (2012). Are there age-related differences in the song repertoire size of Eurasian blackbirds? Acta Ethologica, 15(2), 203-210. |
|
Hesler, N., Mundry, R., Sacher, T., Coppack, T., Bairlein, F., & Dabelsteen, T. (2012). Song repertoire size correlates with measures of body size in Eurasian blackbirds. Behaviour, 149(6), 645-665. |
|
Hopkins, W., Pika, S., Liebal, K., Bania, A., Meguerditchian, A., Gardner, M., & Schapiro, S. J. (2012). Handedness for manual gestures in great apes: A meta-analysis. In S. Pika, & K. Liebal ( |
|
Hribar, A., Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2012). Children's reasoning about spatial relational similarity: The effect of alignment and relational complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 490-500. |
|
Ibbotson, P., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Analogical mapping in construction learning. In P. Robinson ( |
|
Ibbotson, P., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Semantics of the transitive construction: Prototype effects and developmental comparisons. Cognitive Science, 36(7), 1268-1288. |
|
Junker, J., Blake, S., Boesch, C., Campbell, G., du Toit, L., Duvall, C., Ekobo, A., Etoga, G., Galat-Luong, A., Gamys, J., Ganas-Swaray, J., Gatti, S., Ghiurghi, A., Granier, N., Hart, J., Head, J. S., Herbinger, I., Hicks, T. C., Huijbregts, B., Imong, I. S., Kuempel, N., Lahm, S., Lindsell, J., Maisels, F., McLennan, M., Martinez, L., Morgan, B., Morgan, D., Mulindahabi, F., Mundry, R., N'Goran, P. K., Normand, E., Ntongho, A., Okon, D. T., Petre, C.-A., Plumptre, A., Rainey, H., Regnaut, S., Sanz, C., Stokes, E., Tondossama, A., Tranquilli, S., Sunderland-Groves, J., Walsh, P., Warren, Y., Williamson, E. A., & Kuehl, H. S. (2012). Recent decline in suitable environmental conditions for African great apes. Diversity and Distributions, 18(11), 1077-1091. |
|
Kaiser, I., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Theft in an ultimatum game: chimpanzees and bonobos are insensitive to unfairness. Biology Letters, 8(6), 942-945. |
|
Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Developmental Science, 15(2), 222-232. |
|
Kano, F., Call, J., & Tomonaga, M. (2012). Face and eye scanning in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), orangutans (Pongo abelii), and humans (Homo sapiens): Unique eye-viewing patterns in humans among hominids. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(4), 388-398. |
|
Karg, K., & Sen, S. (2012). Gene × Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics. In Behavioral Neurogenetics (pp. 441-462). Heidelberg: Springer. |
|
Kirchhofer, K. C., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Dogs (Canis familiaris), but Not Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), Understand Imperative Pointing. PLoS One, 7(2): e30913. |
|
Kirschner, S. (2012). Skip the age of playback. Emotion Review, 4(3), 285-286. |
|
Krajewski, G., Lieven, E. V. M., & Theakston, A. L. (2012). Productivity of a Polish child's inflectional noun morphology: a naturalistic study. Morphology, 22(1), 9-34. |
|
Kulik, L., Muniz, L., Mundry, R., & Widdig, A. (2012). Patterns of interventions and the effect of coalitions and sociality on male fitness. Molecular Ecology, 21(3), 699-714. |
|
Lallée, S., Pattacini, U., Lemaignan, S., Lenz, A., Melhuish, C., Natale, L., Skachek, S., Hamann, K., Steinwender, J., Sisbot, E., Metta, G., Guitton, J., Alami, R., Warnier, M., Pipe, T., Warneken, F., & Dominey, P. (2012). Towards a Platform-Independent Cooperative Human Robot Interaction System: III An Architecture for Learning and Executing Actions and Shared Plans. IEEE transactions on autonomous mental development, 4(3), 239-253. |
|
Liebal, K., & Call, J. (2012). The origins of non-human primates' manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 118-128. |
|
Liebal, K., & Haun, D. B. M. (2012). The importance of comparative psychology for developmental science. International Journal of Developmental Science, 6, 21-23. |
|
Liebal, K., & Kaminski, J. (2012). Gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, H. moloch, H. lar, Symphalangus syndactylus) follow human gaze, but do not take the visual perspective of others. Animal Cognition, 15(6), 1211-1216. |
|
Luef, E. M., & Liebal, K. (2012). Infant-directed communication in lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Do older animals scaffold communicative competence in infants? American Journal of Primatology, 74(9), 841-852. |
|
Luncz, L. V., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2012). Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Current Biology, 22(10), 922-926. |
|
Martin Ordas, G., Schumacher, L., & Call, J. (2012). Sequential Tool Use in Great Apes. PLoS One, 7(12): e52074. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., Jaeck, F., & Call, J. (2012). Barriers and traps: great apes' performance in two functionally equivalent tasks. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 1007-1013. |
|
Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Origins of the human pointing gesture: a training study. Developmental Science, 15(6), 817-829. |
|
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(2), 184-210. |
|
Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., & Silk, J. B. ( |
|
Moll, H. (2012). Von der Teilnahme an gemeinsamer Aufmerksamkeit zum Begriff von Perspektiven: Stufen einer Entwicklung. In P. Stekeler-Weithofer ( |
|
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2012). Joint Attention as the Fundamental Basis of Understanding Perspectives. In A. Seemann ( |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Three-Year-Olds Understand Appearance and Reality - Just Not About the Same Object at the Same Time. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1124-1132. |
|
N'Goran, P. K., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., N'Goran, E. K., Herbinger, I., Yapi, F. A., & Kühl, H. (2012). Hunting, law enforcement, and African primate conservation. Conservation Biology, 26(3), 565-571. |
|
Nelson, E., McIntyre, M. H., Rakhovskaya, M., Wobber, V., Herrmann, E., & Hare, B. (2012). Variation in 2D:4D between captive and free-ranging primates: implications for digit ratio research. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 147(Suppl. 54), 222-222. |
|
Nitzschner, M., Melis, A. P., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Dogs (Canis familiaris) Evaluate Humans on the Basis of Direct Experiences Only. PLoS One, 7(10): e46880. |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Imitative learning in humans and animals. In N. M. Seel ( |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182-192. |
|
Pradhan, G. R., Tennie, C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology in apes and hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 63(1), 180-190. |
|
Rakoczy, H., & Haun, D. B. M. (2012). Vor- und nichtsprachliche Kognition. In W. Schneider, & U. Lindenberger ( |
|
Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(37), 14824-14829. |
|
Rossano, F. (2012). Gaze behavior in face-to-face interaction. PhD Thesis, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen. |
|
Rossano, F. (2012). Gaze in conversation. In J. Sidnell, & T. Stivers ( |
|
Rossano, F., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). One-year-old infants follow others' voice direction. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1298-1302. |
|
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125(1), 49-63. |
|
Sabbatini, G., Manrique, H. M., Call, J., & Visalberghi, E. (2012). Do Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Select Tools on the Basis of Rigidity? Folia primatologica, 82, 265-265. |
|
Sabbatini, G., Truppa, V., Hribar, A., Gambetta, B., Call, J., & Visalberghi, E. (2012). Understanding the functional properties of tools: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) attend to tool features differently. Animal Cognition, 15(4), 577-590. |
|
Schirmer, A. F. (2012). Unable or unwilling?: Cognitive and motivational foundations of collaborative activities in primates. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H. (2012). Ontogenetic origins of children's appreciation for normativity. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young Children Enforce Social Norms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 232-236. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation. Cognition, 124(3), 325-333. |
|
Schneider, A.-C., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How chimpanzees solve collective action problems. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1749), 4946-4954. |
|
Schneider, C., Call, J., & Liebal, K. (2012). Onset and early use of gestural communication in nonhuman great apes. American Journal of Primatology, 74(2), 102-113. |
|
Schneider, C., Call, J., & Liebal, K. (2012). What role do mothers play in the gestural acquisition of Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? International Journal of Primatology, 33(1), 246-262. |
|
Schrauf, C., Call, J., Fuwa, K., & Hirata, S. (2012). Do Chimpanzees Use Weight to Select Hammer Tools? PLoS One, 7(7): e41044. |
|
Seed, A., Seddon, E., Greene, B., & Call, J. (2012). Chimpanzee 'folk physics': bringing failures into focus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2743-2752. |
|
Sommer, C., Todt, D., Ostreiher, R., & Mundry, R. (2012). Urgency-related alarm calling in Arabian babblers, Turdoides squamiceps: predator distance matters in the use of alarm call types. Behaviour, 149(7), 755-773. |
|
Stivers, T., & Rossano, F. (2012). Mobilizing response in interaction: A compositional view of questions. In J. P. De Ruiter ( |
|
Stoll, S., Bickel, B., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Banjade, G., Bhatta, T. N., Gaenszle, M., Pettigrew, J., Rai, I. P., Rai, M., & Rai, N. K. (2012). Nouns and verbs in Chintang: children's usage and surrounding adult speech. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 284-321. |
|
Stumper, B. (2012). The usage-based approach to language acquisition: Evidence from German. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Tempelmann, S., & Liebal, K. (2012). Spontaneous use of gesture sequences in orangutans: A case for strategy? In S. Pika, & K. Liebal ( |
|
Tennie, C. (2012). Punishing for your own good: The case of reputation-based cooperation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 40-41. |
|
Tennie, C., & Over, H. (2012). Cultural intelligence is key to explaining human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(4), 242-243. |
|
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Untrained Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Fail to Imitate Novel Actions. PLoS One, 7(8): e41548. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Maslen, R., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). The acquisition of the active transitive construction in English: A detailed case study. Cognitive Linguistics, 23(1), 91-128. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2012). Why be nice? Better not think about it. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 580-581. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Hamann, K. (2012). Collaboration in young children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 1-12. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Hamann, K.(2012). Kooperation bei Kleinkindern. Retrieved from https://www.mpg.de/4658054/Kooperation_bei_Kleinkindern?c=5732343. |
|
Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673-692. |
|
Tranquilli, S., Abedi-Lartey, M., Amsini, F., Arranz, L., Asamoah, A., Babafemi, O., Barakabuye, N., Campbell, G., Chancellor, R., Davenport, T. R., Dunn, A., Dupain, J., Ellis, C., Etoga, G., Furuichi, T., Gatti, S., Ghiurghi, A., Greengrass, E., Hashimoto, C., Hart, J., Herbinger, I., Hicks, T. C., Holbech, L. H., Huijbregts, B., Imong, I., Kumpel, N., Maisels, F., Marshall, P., Nixon, S., Normand, E., Nziguyimpa, L., Nzooh-Dogmo, Z., Okon, D. T., Plumptre, A., Rundus, A., Sunderland-Groves, J., Todd, A., Warren, Y., Mundry, R., Boesch, C., & Kühl, H. S. (2012). Lack of conservation effort rapidly increases African great ape extinction risk. Conservation Letters, 5(1), 48-55. |
|
Van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Haun, D. B. M., Mundry, R., & Bodamer, M. D. (2012). Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 4362-4367. |
|
Völter, C., & Call, J. (2012). Problem solving in great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo abelii): the effect of visual feedback. Animal Cognition, 15(5), 923-936. |
|
Warneken, F., & Melis, A. P. (2012). The Ontogeny and Phylogeny of Cooperation. In J. Vonk, & T. K. Shackelford ( |
|
Warneken, F., Graefenhain, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children’s understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. Developmental Science, 15(1), 54-61. |
|
Whitehouse, H., & Cohen, E. (2012). Seeking a Rapprochement Between Anthropology and the Cognitive Sciences: A Problem-Driven Approach. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(3), 404-412. |
|
Wyman, E., Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Reply to comments. Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation: The Interdependence Hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 689-690. |
2011
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, V., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2011). Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens). Animal Cognition, 14(5), 695-706. |
|
Acerbi, A., Tennie, C., & Nunn, C. L. (2011). Modeling imitation and emulation in constrained search spaces. Learning & Behavior, 39(2), 104-114. |
|
Ambridge, B., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2011). Child language acquisition: contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. |
|
Barelli, C., Reichard, U. H., & Mundry, R. (2011). Is grooming used as a commodity in wild white-handed gibbons, Hylobates lar? Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 801-809. |
|
Brandt, S., & Kidd, E. (2011). Relative clause acquisition and representation: Evidence from spontaneous speech, sentence repetition, and comprehension. In D. Schönefeld ( |
|
Brandt, S., Verhagen, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). German children's productivity with simple transitive and complement-clause constructions: Testing the effects of frequency and diversity. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(2), 325-357. |
|
Bräuer, J., & Call, J. (2011). The magic cup: great apes and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) individuate objects according to their properties. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(3), 353-361. |
|
Bullinger, A. F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over collaborative strategies towards goals. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 1135-1141. |
|
Bullinger, A. F., Wyman, E., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Coordination of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a stag hunt game. International Journal of Primatology, 32(6), 1296-1310. |
|
Bullinger, A. F., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Different social motives in the gestural communication of chimpanzees and human children. Developmental Science, 14(1), 58-68. |
|
Burrows, A. M., Diogo, R., Waller, B. M., Bonar, C. J., & Liebal, K. (2011). Evolution of the muscles of facial expression in a monogamous ape: Evaluating the relative influences of ecological and phylogenetic factors in Hylobatids. The Anatomical Record: Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology, 294(4), 645-663. |
|
Call, J. (2011). How Artificial Communication Affects the Communication and Cognition of the Great Apes. Mind & Language, 26(1), 1-20. |
|
Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early Social Cognition in Three Cultural Contexts. Boston: Wiley-Blackwell. |
|
Carpenter, M. (2011). Social cognition and social motivations in infancy. In U. Goswami ( |
|
Cohen, E., Burdett, E., Knight, N., & Barrett, J. (2011). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in person-body reasoning: experimental evidence from the United Kingdom and Brazilian Amazon. Cognitive Science, 35(7), 1282-1304. |
|
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Children aged 2; 1 use transitive syntax to make a semantic-role interpretation in a pointing task. Journal of Child Language, 38(5), 1109-1123. |
|
Epp, J. R., Scott, N. A., & Galea, L. A. M. (2011). Strain differences in neurogenesis and activation of new neurons in the dentate gyrus in response to spatial learning. Neuroscience, 172, 342-354. |
|
Fedurek, P., & Slocombe, K. E. (2011). Primate vocal communication: A useful tool for understanding human speech and language evolution? Human Biology, 83(2), 153-173. |
|
Fletcher, G. E. (2011). Collaborative activities in children and chimpanzees: The different cognitive mechanisms underlying human and chimpanzee collaboration. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Fürtbauer, I., Mundry, R., Heistermann, M., Schülke, O., & Ostner, J. (2011). You mate, I mate: Macaque females synchronize sex not cycles. PLoS ONE, 6(10): e26144. |
|
Grossmann, T., Johnson, M. H., Vaish, A., Hughes, D. A., Quinque, D., Stoneking, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Genetic and neural dissociation of individual responses to emotional expressions in human infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 57-66. |
|
Grote, S., Call, J., & Kivell, T. L. (2011). Who is more bipedal? Positional behaviour in captive bonobos and chimpanzees. |
|
Grünloh, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). German children use prosody to identify participant roles in transitive sentences. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(2), 393-419. |
|
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476(7360), 328-331. |
|
Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2011). Chimpanzee problem-solving: contrasting the use of causal and arbitrary cues. Animal Cognition, 14(6), 871-878. |
|
Hanus, D., Mendes, N., Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2011). Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE, 6(6): e19555. |
|
Haun, D. B. M. (2011). How odd I am! In M. Brockman ( |
|
Haun, D. B. M. (2011). Memory for body movements in Namibian hunter-gatherer children. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10(1), 56-62. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82(6), 1759-1767. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Jordan, F. M., Vallortigara, G., & Clayton, N. S. (2011). Origins of spatial, temporal and numerical cognition: insights from comparative psychology. In S. Dehaene, & E. M. Brannon ( |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Nawroth, C., & Call, J. (2011). Great apes' risk-taking strategies in a decision making task. PLoS ONE, 6(12): e28801. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Rapold, C. J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2011). Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures. Cognition, 119(1), 70-80. |
|
Hepach, R., Kliemann, D., Grüneisen, S., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2011). Conceptualizing emotions along the dimensions of valence, arousal, and communicative frequency: Implications for social-cognitive tests and training tools. Frontiers in Psychology, 2: 266. |
|
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1393-1405. |
|
Hesler, N., Mundry, R., & Dabelsteen, T. (2011). Does song repertoire size in Common Blackbirds play a role in an intra-sexual context? Journal of Ornithology, 152(3), 591-601. |
|
Hribar, A., & Call, J. (2011). Great apes use landmark cues over spatial relations to find hidden food. Animal Cognition, 14(5), 623-635. |
|
Hribar, A., Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2011). Great apes' strategies to map spatial relations. Animal Cognition, 14(4), 511-523. |
|
Ibbotson, P., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). The Role of Pronoun Frames in Early Comprehension of Transitive Constructions in English. Language Learning and Development, 7(1), 24-39. |
|
Jorschick, L., Quick, A. E., Glässer, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). German-English-speaking children's mixed NPs with 'correct' agreement. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(2), 173-183. |
|
Kaminski, J. (2011). Communicative cues among and between human and non-human primates: attending to specificity in triadic gestural interactions. In M. Boos ( |
|
Kaminski, J., & Bräuer, J. (2011). So klug ist Ihr Hund. Stuttgart: Kosmos. |
|
Kaminski, J., Neumann, M., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 651-658. |
|
Kaminski, J., Nitzschner, M., Wobber, V., Tennie, C., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts? Animal Behaviour, 81(1), 195-203. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Santos, L. R., Hood, B. M., & Call, J. (2011). The Limits of Endowment Effects in Great Apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(4), 436-445. |
|
Kanngiesser, P., Sueur, C., Riedl, K., Großmann, J., & Call, J. (2011). Grooming Network Cohesion and the Role of Individuals in a Captive Chimpanzee Group. American Journal of Primatology, 73(8), 758-767. |
|
Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomonaga, M. (2011). The visual strategy specific to humans among hominids: A study using the gap–overlap paradigm. Vision Research, 51(23-24), 2348-2355. |
|
Kirjavainen, M., & Lieven, E. (2011). Acquisition of relative clauses in Finnish: The effect of input. In E. Kidd ( |
|
Kirschner, S. (2011). The social origins of music. PhD Thesis, Humboldt-Univ., Berlin. |
|
Kokal, I., Engel, A., Kirschner, S., & Keysers, C. (2011). Synchronized drumming enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment - if the rhythm comes easily. PLoS ONE, 6(11): e27272. |
|
Krajewski, G., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). How Polish children switch from one case to another when using novel nouns: Challenges for models of inflectional morphology. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4-6), 830-861. |
|
Lallée, S., Pattacini, U., Boucher, J. D., Lemaignan, S., Lenz, A., Melhuish, C., Natale, L., Skachek, S., Hamann, K., Steinwender, J., Sisbot, E. A., Metta, G., Alami, R., Warnier, M., Guitton, J., Warneken, F., & Dominey, P. F. (2011). Towards a platform-independent cooperative human-robot interaction system: II. Perception, execution and imitation of goal directed actions. In 2011 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (pp. 2895-2902). |
|
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children's understanding of markedness in non-verbal communication. Journal of Child Language, 38(4), 888-903. |
|
Lieven, E. V. M., & Behrens, H. (2011). Dense Sampling. In E. Hoff ( |
|
Lieven, E., & Brandt, S. (2011). The constructivist approach = La perspectiva constructivista. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34(3), 281-297. |
|
Lieven, E., & Brandt, S. (2011). The constructivist approach: reply to commentaries = Respuesta a los comentarios de La perspectiva constructivista. debate: the debate on language acquisition: constructivism versus innatism / el debate sobre la adquisición del lenguaje: constructivismo versus innatismo, 319-322. |
|
Lieven, E., & Noble, C. (2011). The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Zeitschrift für Anglistik und Amerikanistik, 59(4), 411-424. |
|
Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 16-29. |
|
Manrique, H. M., & Call, J. (2011). Spontaneous use of tools as straws in great apes. Animal Cognition, 14(2), 213-226. |
|
Manrique, H. M., Sabbatini, G., Call, J., & Visalberghi, E. (2011). Tool choice on the basis of rigidity in capuchin monkeys. Animal Cognition, 14(6), 775-786. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., & Call, J. (2011). Luces y sombras en el pensamiento de Darwin en relación con la evolución cognitiva de las especies. In G. Gutiérrez, & M. R. Papini ( |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., & Call, J. (2011). Memory processing in great apes: the effect of time and sleep. Biology Letters, 7(6), 829-832. |
|
Melis, A. P., Schneider, A.-C., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, share food in the same way after collaborative and individual food acquisition. Animal Behaviour, 82(3), 485-493. |
|
Melis, A. P., Warneken, F., Jensen, K., Schneider, A.-C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 278(1710), 1405-1413. |
|
Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., & Call, J. (2011). Primates do not spontaneously use shape properties for object individuation: a competence or a performance problem? Animal Cognition, 14(3), 407-414. |
|
Mersmann, D., Tomasello, M., Call, J., Kaminski, J., & Taborsky, M. (2011). Simple Mechanisms Can Explain Social Learning in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris). Ethology, 117(8), 675-690. |
|
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How Does It Look? Level 2 Perspective-Taking at 36 Months of Age. Child Development, 82(2), 661-673. |
|
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Perspective-Taking and its Foundation in Joint Attention. In J. Roessler, H. Lerman, & N. Eilan ( |
|
Moll, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Social Engagement Leads 2-Year-Olds to Overestimate Others’ Knowledge. Infancy, 16(3), 248-265. |
|
Moore, R. (2011). [Book Review of:] Mindreaders: the cognitive basis of 'theory of mind' / Ian Apperly. - Hove/New York, NY: Psychology Press, 2011. - 219 pp. ISBN 1-84169-697-8 ISBN 978-1-84169-697-3. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 18(11/12), 232-238. |
|
Mundry, R. (2011). Issues in information theory-based statistical inference - a commentary from a frequentist's perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(1), 57-68. |
|
Pettersson, H., Kaminski, J., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 133(3-4), 235-245. |
|
Rekers, Y., Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Current Biology, 21(20), 1756-1758. |
|
Rossano, F., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition, 121(2), 219-227. |
|
Salomo, D., Graf, E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). The role of perceptual availability and discourse context in young children's question answering. Journal of Child Language, 38(4), 918-931. |
|
Sauter, D. A., LeGuen, O., & Haun, D. B. M. (2011). Categorical perception of emotional facial expressions does not require lexical categories. Emotion, 11(6), 1479-1483. |
|
Scheider, L. (2011). The command hypothesis versus the information hypothesis: How do domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) comprehend the human pointing gesture? PhD Thesis, Freie Univ., Berlin. |
|
Scheider, L., Grassmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when following a human pointing gesture. PLoS ONE, 6(7): e21676. |
|
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7), 3077-3079. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants. PLoS One, 6(10): e23223. |
|
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science, 14(3), 530-539. |
|
Schrauf, C., & Call, J. (2011). Great Apes Use Weight as a Cue to Find Hidden Food. American Journal of Primatology, 73(4), 323-334. |
|
Schrauf, C., Call, J., & Pauen, S. (2011). The Effect of Plausible Versus Implausible Balance Scale Feedback on the Expectancies of 3- to 4-Year-Old Children. Journal of Cognition & Development, 12(4), 518-536. |
|
Seed, A., Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2011). Causal Knowledge In Corvids, Primates and Children: More Than Meets The Eye? In T. McCormack, C. Hoerl, & S. Butterfill ( |
|
Slocombe, K. E., Waller, B. M., & Liebal, K. (2011). The language void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 81(5), 919-924. |
|
Steinwender, J. (2011). Young children's collaborative problem-solving. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Stumper, B. (2011). Kookkurrenzen in der Kind-gerichteten Sprache: Verlässlicher Hinweis auf die Wortarten? In A. Lasch, & A. Ziem ( |
|
Stumper, B., Bannard, C., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2011). "Frequent Frames" in German Child-Directed Speech: A Limited Cue to Grammatical Categories. Cognitive Science, 35(6), 1190-1205. |
|
Surbeck, M., Mundry, R., & Hohmann, G. (2011). Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos (Pan paniscus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 278(1705), 590-598. |
|
Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., & Liebal, K. (2011). Focus on the essential: All great apes know when others are being attentive. Animal Cognition, 14(3), 433-439. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2011). Ernst von Glasersfeld: Some “Partial Memories”. Constructivist Foundations, 6(2), 164-165. Retrieved from http://constructivist.info/6/2/164. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2011). Language development. In U. Goswami ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2011). Methodological Challenges in the Study of Primate Cognition. Science, 334(6060), 1227-1228. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2011). Replik auf die Kommentare. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Philosophie, 59(1), 164-169. |
|
Vaish, A., & Warneken, F. (2011). Social-Cognitive Contributors to Young Children's Empathic and Prosocial Behavior. In J. Decety ( |
|
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young Children’s Responses to Guilt Displays. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1248-1262. |
|
Vaish, A., Demir, Ö. E., & Baldwin, D. (2011). Thirteen- and 18-month-old Infants Recognize When They Need Referential Information. Social Development, 20(3), 431-449. |
|
Vaish, A., Missana, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 124-130. |
|
Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young Children Share the Spoils After Collaboration. Psychological Science, 22(2), 267-273. |
|
Wittig, M. (2011). Selfish versus prosocial preferences: Using game theory to investigate young children's preferences in resource distribution. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Wobber, V., & Kaminski, J. (2011). What do Dogs Understand About Human Communicative Signals?: A Novel Synthesis. In V. M. DeGiovine ( |
|
Wobber, V., Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Wrangham, R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Species differences in the rate of cognitive ontogeny among humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 144(S52), 313-314. |
|
Wyman, E., & Rakoczy, H. (2011). Social conventions, institutions and human uniqueness: lessons from children and chimpanzees. In W. Welsch, W. J. Singer, & A. Wunder ( |
2010
Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The Influence of Frequency and Semantic Similarity on How Children Learn Grammar. First Language, 30(1), 79-101. |
|
Abraham, A., Rakoczy, H., Werning, M., Cramon, D. Y. v., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Matching mind to world and vice versa: Functional dissociations between belief and desire mental state processing. Social Neuroscience, 5(1), 1-18. |
|
Addis, L., Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., Sabisch, B., Barry, J., Richter, N., Ludwig, A. A., Rübsamen, R., Albert, F. W., Pääbo, S., Newbury, D. F., & Monaco, A. P. (2010). A locus for an auditory processing deficit and language impairment in an extended pedigree maps to 12p13.31-q14.3. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 9(6), 545-561. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Call, J., & Barth, J. (2010). Great apes track hidden objects after changes in the objects' position and in subject's orientation. American Journal of Primatology, 72(4), 349-359. |
|
Amici, F., Aureli, F., & Call, J. (2010). Monkeys and apes: Are their cognitive skills really so different? American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 143(2), 188-197. |
|
Bickel, B., Rai, M., Paudyal, N. P., Banjade, G., Bhatta, T. N., Gaenszle, M., Lieven, E., Rai, I. P., Rai, N. K., & Stoll, S. (2010). The syntax of three-argument verbs in Chintang and Belhare (Southeastern Kiranti). In A. L. Malchukov, M. Haspelmath, & B. Comrie ( |
|
Boos, K., Gutow, L., Mundry, R., & Franke, H.-D. (2010). Sediment preference and burrowing behaviour in the sympatric brittlestars Ophiura albida Forbes, 1839 and Ophiura ophiura (Linnaeus, 1758) (Ophiuroidea, Echinodermata). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 393(1-2), 176-181. |
|
Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Development of word order in German complement-clause constructions: Effects of input frequencies, lexical items, and discourse function. Language, 86(3), 583-610. |
|
Cacchione, T., & Call, J. (2010). Do gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) fail to represent objects in the context of cohesion violations? Cognition, 116(2), 193-203. |
|
Cacchione, T., & Call, J. (2010). Intuitions about gravity and solidity in great apes: the tubes task. Developmental Science, 13(2), 320-330. |
|
Call, J. (2010). Do apes know that they could be wrong? Animal Cognition, 13(5), 689-700. |
|
Call, J. (2010). Do the great apes make donations? In F. Adloff, E. Priller, & R. G. Strachwitz ( |
|
Call, J. (2010). Interview: Josep Call. Current Biology, 20(8), R343-R343. |
|
Call, J. (2010). Trapping the minds of apes: Causal knowledge and inferential reasoning about object-object interactions. In E. V. Lonsdorf, S. R. Ross, & T. Matsuzawa ( |
|
Call, J. (2010). Understanding apes to understand humans: The case of object-object relations. In B. Glatzeder, V. Goel, & A. Müller ( |
|
Chan, A., Meints, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children's comprehension of English SVO word order revisited: Testing the same children in act-out and intermodal preferential looking tasks. Cognitive Development, 25(1), 30-45. |
|
Cheney, D. L., Moscovice, L. R., Heesen, M., Mundry, R., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2010). Contingent cooperation between wild female baboons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(21), 9562-9566. |
|
Cohen, E. (2010). Anthropology of knowledge. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 16(Supplement 1), S193-S202. |
|
Duschek, S., Heiss, H., Schmidt, M. F. H., Werner, N. S., & Schuepbach, D. (2010). Interactions between systemic hemodynamics and cerebral blood flow during attentional processing. Psychophysiology, 47(6), 1159-1166. |
|
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds' attention to a contextually new referent. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(11), 3098-3105. |
|
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13(1), 252-263. |
|
Greenberg, J. R., Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 873-880. |
|
Grosse, G., Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Behne, T. (2010). Infants Communicate in Order to Be Understood. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1710-1722. |
|
Grosse, G., Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2010). 21-Month-olds understand the cooperative logic of requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(12), 3377-3383. |
|
Hare, B., Rosati, A. G., Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The domestication hypothesis for dogs' skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and, Wynne et al. (2008). Animal Behaviour, 79(2), e1-e6. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Jordan, F. M., Vallortigara, G., & Clayton, N. S. (2010). Origins of spatial, temporal and numerical cognition: Insights from comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 552-560. |
|
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. PLoS ONE, 5(8): e12438. |
|
Herrmann, E., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21(1), 102-110. |
|
Hohmann, G., Potts, K., N'Guessan, A. K., Fowler, A., Mundry, R., Ganzhorn, J. U., & Ortmann, S. (2010). Plant foods consumed by Pan: Exploring the variation of nutritional ecology across Africa. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 141(3), 476-485. |
|
Jensen, K. (2010). Punishment and spite, the dark side of cooperation. Cooperation and deception: from evolution to mechanisms, 2635-2650. |
|
Jensen, K., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Punishment. In Michael D. Breed and Janice Moore ( |
|
Kaminski, J., & Emery, N. J. (2010). Social cognition and theory of mind. In Michael D. Breed and Janice Moore ( |
|
Kanngiesser, P., & Call, J. (2010). Bonobos, chimpanzees, gorillas, and orang utans use feature and spatial cues in two spatial memory tasks. Animal Cognition, 13(3), 419-430. |
|
Kidd, E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Lexical frequency and exemplar-based learning effects in language acquisition: evidence from sentential complements. Language Sciences, 32(1), 132-142. |
|
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354-364. |
|
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). A New Change-of-Contents False Belief Test: Children and Chimpanzees Compared. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(2), 145-165. |
|
Lallee, S., Yoshida, E., Mallet, A., Nori, F., Natale, L., Metta, G., Warneken, F., & Dominey, F. (2010). Human-Robot Cooperation Based on Interaction Learning. In Olivier Sigaud; Jan Peters ( |
|
Lenz, A., Skachek, S., Hamann, K., Steinwender, J., Pipe, A. G., & Melhuish, C. (2010). The BERT2 infrastructure: An integrated system for the study of human-robot interaction. In Robotics and Automation Society ( |
|
Liebal, K. (2010). Infants' and young children's understanding of common ground and markedness in communication (Zugl.: Leipzig, Univ., Diss., 2009). Leipzig: Universitätsverlag Leipzig. |
|
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Infants' Use of Shared Experience in Declarative Pointing. Infancy, 15(5), 545-556. |
|
Lieven, E. (2010). Bilingual Language Acquisition: Commentary on Sebastian-Galles. Human Development, 53(5), 256-263. |
|
Lieven, E. (2010). Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua, 120(11), 2546-2556. |
|
Lieven, E. (2010). Language development in a cross-linguistic context. In M. Kail, & M. Hickmann ( |
|
Lieven, E., & Stoll, S. (2010). Language. In Marc H. Bornstein ( |
|
Manrique, H. M., Gross, A.-N.-M., & Call, J. (2010). Great Apes Select Tools on the Basis of Their Rigidity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36(4), 409-422. |
|
Martin Ordas, G., Haun, D. B. M., Colmenares, F., & Call, J. (2010). Keeping track of time: Evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition, 13(2), 331-340. |
|
Matthews, D., & Bannard, C. (2010). Children’s Production of Unfamiliar Word Sequences Is Predicted by Positional Variability and Latent Classes in a Large Sample of Child-Directed Speech. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 465-488. |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). What’s in a Manner of Speaking? Children’s Sensitivity to Partner-Specific Referential Precedents. Developmental Psychology, 46(4), 749-760. |
|
Melis, A. P., & Semmann, D. (2010). How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 365(1553), 2663-2674. |
|
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). 36-month-olds conceal visual and auditory information from others. Developmental Science, 13(3), 479-489. |
|
Melis, A. P., Warneken, F., & Hare, B. (2010). Collaboration and helping in chimpanzees. In Elizabeth V. Lonsdorf, Stephen R. Ross, & Tetsuro Matsuzawa ( |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Infant cognition. Current Biology, 20(20), R872-R875. |
|
Moore, R. (2010). [Book Review]: Michelangelo's Finger: An Exploration of Everyday Transcendence / Raymond Tallis. - New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. - xxii, 166 pp. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17(5-6), 238-243. |
|
Over, H., & Gattis, M. (2010). Verbal imitation is based on intention understanding. Cognitive Development, 25(1), 46-55. |
|
Pelé, M., Thierry, B., Call, J., & Dufour, V. (2010). Monkeys fail to reciprocate in an exchange task. Animal Cognition, 13(5), 745-751. |
|
Potì, P., Kanngiesser, P., Saporiti, M., Amiconi, A., Bläsing, B., & Call, J. (2010). Searching in the Middle—Capuchins' (Cebus apella) and Bonobos' (Pan paniscus) Behavior During a Spatial Search Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36(1), 92-109. |
|
Pyykkönen, P., Matthews, D., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Three-year-olds are sensitive to semantic prominence during online language comprehension: A visual world study of pronoun resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(1), 115-129. |
|
Quillfeldt, P., Poisbleau, M., Mundry, R., & Masello, J. F. (2010). Are acoustical parameters of begging call elements of thin-billed prions related to chick condition? Acta Ethologica, 13(1), 1-9. |
|
Rabanal, L. I., Kuehl, H., Mundry, R., Robbins, M. M., & Boesch, C. (2010). Oil prospecting and its impact on large rainforest mammals in Loango National Park, Gabon. Biological Conservation, 143(4), 1017-1024. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2010). Executive function and the development of belief–desire psychology. Developmental Science, 13(4), 648-661. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Bigger knows better: Young children selectively learn rule games from adults rather than from peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 785-798. |
|
Ros, R., Lemaignan, S., Sisbot, E. A., Steinwender, J., Hamann, K., & Warneken, F. (2010). Which one? Grounding the referent based on efficient human-robot interaction. In RO-MAN, 2010 IEEE (pp. 570-575). IEEE explore Digital Library. |
|
Ros, R., Sisbot, E. A., Alami, R., Steinwender, J., Hamann, K., & Warneken, F. (2010). Solving ambiguities with perspective taking. In 2010 5th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI 2010) (pp. 181-182). Piscataway, NJ: IEEE. |
|
Rossano, F. (2010). Questioning and responding in Italian. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(10), 2756-2771. |
|
Salomo, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children’s sensitivity to new and given information when answering predicate-focus questions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(1), 101-115. |
|
Sauter, D. (2010). More Than Happy: The Need for Disentangling Positive Emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 36-40. |
|
Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Reply to Gewald: Isolated Himba settlements still exist in Kaokoland. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(18): E76. |
|
Schneider, C., Call, J., & Liebal, K. (2010). Do bonobos say NO by shaking their head? Primates, 51(3), 199-202. |
|
Seed, A., & Call, J. (2010). Problem-solving in tool-using and non-tool-using animals. In Encyclopedia of animal behavior (pp. 778-785). Oxford: Academic Press. |
|
Seed, A., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Primate Cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(3), 407-419. |
|
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Call, J., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Chimpanzees extract social information from agonistic screams. PLoS ONE, 5(7): e11473. |
|
Sprau, P., & Mundry, R. (2010). Song type sharing in common nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos, and its implications for cultural evolution. Animal Behaviour, 80(3), 427-434. |
|
Stivers, T., & Rossano, F. (2010). A Scalar View of Response Relevance. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 43(1), 49-56. |
|
Stivers, T., & Rossano, F. (2010). Mobilizing Response. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 43(1), 3-31. |
|
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE, 5(5): e10544. |
|
Tennie, C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Reputation management in the age of the world-wide web. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(11), 482-488. |
|
Tennie, C., Greve, K., Gretscher, H., & Call, J. (2010). Two-year-old children copy more reliably and more often than nonhuman great apes in multiple observational learning tasks. Primates, 51(4), 337-351. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2010). Human Culture in Evolutionary Perspective. In M. J. Gelfand, & C.-y. Chiu ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2010). Chimpanzee Social Cognition. In Elizabeth V. Lonsdorf, Stephen R. Ross & Tetsuro Matsuzawa ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Herrmann, E. (2010). Ape and human cognition: What's the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 3-8. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. In P. Kappeler, & J. Silk ( |
|
Vaish, A., & Woodward, A. (2010). Infants use attention but not emotions to predict others’ actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(1), 79-87. |
|
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young Children Selectively Avoid Helping People With Harmful Intentions. Child Development, 81(6), 1661-1669. |
|
Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Hare, B., & Call, J. (2010). Reaching around barriers: the performance of the great apes and 3–5-year-old children. Animal Cognition, 13(2), 273-285. |
|
Vogt, P., & Lieven, E. (2010). Verifying theories of language acquisition using computer models of language evolution. Adaptive Behavior, 18(1), 21-35. |
|
Whiten, A. W., McGrew, W. C., Aiello, L. C., Boesch, C., Boyd, R., Byrne, R. W., Dunbar, R. I. M., Matsuzawa, T., Silk, J. B., Tomasello, M., Schaik, C. P. v., & Wrangham, R. (2010). Studying Extant Species to Model Our Past [Letter to the Editor]. Science, 327(5964), 410-410. |
|
Yoon, J. M. D., & Tennie, C. (2010). Contagious yawning: a reflection of empathy, mimicry, or contagion? Animal Behaviour, 12(4), e1-e3. |
|
Zmyj, N., Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Daum, M. M. (2010). The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(4), 208-220. |
|
Zuberbühler, K., & Janmaat, K. (2010). Foraging cognition in nonhuman primates. In Michael L. Platt and Asif A. Ghazanfar ( |
2009
Amici, F., Aureli, F., Visalberghi, E., & Call, J. (2009). Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Follow Gaze Around Barriers: Evidence for Perspective Taking? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(4), 368-374. |
|
Amici, F., Call, J., & Aureli, F. (2009). Variation in withholding of information in three monkey species. Proceeding of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1671), 3311-3318. |
|
Bannard, C., & Lieven, E. (2009). Repetition and Reuse in Child Language Learning. In Roberta L. Corrigan ( |
|
Bannard, C., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(41), 17284-17289. |
|
Brandt, S., Kidd, E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The discourse bases of relativization: An investigation of young German and English-speaking children's comprehension of relative clauses. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(3), 539-570. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71(2), 175-181. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Do great apes use emotional expressions to infer desires? Developmental Science, 12(5), 688-698. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337-342. |
|
Cacchione, T., Call, J., & Zingg, R. (2009). Gravity and Solidity in Four Great Ape Species (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus): Vertical and Horizontal Variations of the Table Task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(2), 168-180. |
|
Call, J. (2009). Contrasting the Social Cognition of Humans and Nonhuman Apes: The Shared Intentionality Hypothesis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 368-379. |
|
Call, J. (2009). Foreword. In L. S. Röska-Hardy, & E. M. Neumann-Held ( |
|
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Monkeys Like Mimics. Science, 325(5942), 824-825. |
|
Call, J., & Tennie, C. (2009). Animal Culture: Chimpanzee Table Manners? Current Biology, 19(21), R981-R983. |
|
Carpenter, M. (2009). Just how joint is joint action in infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 380-392. |
|
Carpenter, M. (2009). Prelinguistic communication. In Louise Cummings ( |
|
Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2009). Comparing the imitative skills of children and nonhuman apes. Revue de Primatologie, 1(1): 6. Retrieved from http://primatologie.revues.org/263. |
|
Chan, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Children's understanding of the agent-patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(2), 267-300. |
|
Colombi, C., Liebal, K., Tomasello, M., Young, G., Warneken, F., & Rogers, S. J. (2009). Examining correlates of cooperation in autism. Autism, 13(2), 143-163. |
|
Dittmar, M. (2009). Acquiring the transitive construction: The development of understanding word order and case marking cues. PhD Thesis, Freie Univ., Berlin. |
|
Duckworth, A., Masello, J. F., Mundry, R., & Quillfeldt, P. (2009). Functional characterization of begging calls in thin-billed Prions Pachyptila belcheri chicks. Acta Ornithologica, 44(2), 127-137. |
|
Dufour, V., Péle, M., Neumann, M., Thierry, B., & Call, J. (2009). Calculated reciprocity after all: computation behind token transfers in orang-utans. Biology Letters, 5(2), 172-175. |
|
Gladbach, A., Büßer, C., Mundry, R., & Quillfeldt, P. (2009). Acoustic parameters of begging calls indicate chick body condition in Wilson's storm-petrels Oceanites oceanicus. Journal of Ethology, 27(2), 267-274. |
|
Gomes, C. M., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2009). Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 276(1657), 699-706. |
|
Gräfenhain, M. (2009). Young children's understanding of joint activity (Leipzig, Univ., Diss., 2009). Leipzig: Universitätsverlag Leipzig. |
|
Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). One-year-olds’ understanding of nonverbal gestures directed to a third person. Cognitive Development, 24(1), 23-33. |
|
Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young Children's Understanding of Joint Commitments. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 1430-1443. |
|
Grassmann, S., Stracke, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Two-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents of novel words based on pragmatics. Cogntition, 112(3), 488-493. |
|
Große, G. (2009). The cognitive and motivational bases of young children's communication: The case of directives. PhD Thesis, Univ., Manchester. |
|
Grossmann, T., & Vaish, A. (2009). Reading Faces in Infancy: Developing a Multi-Level Analysis of a Social Stimulus. In Tricia Striano; Vincent Reid ( |
|
Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S., Nakamura, K., & Özcaliskan, S. ( |
|
Hanus, D. (2009). Great apes' causal cognition in the physical domain. PhD Thesis, Humboldt-Univ., Berlin. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2009). Great apes’ capacities to recognize relational similarity. Cognition, 110(2), 147-159. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Rapold, C. J. (2009). Variation in memory for body movements across cultures. Current Biology, 19(23), R1068-R1069. |
|
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The origin of human temperament: Differences in the response to novelty among great apes and human children. Poster presented at SRCD Biennial Meeting, Denver, CO. |
|
Hilbig, H., Beil, B., Hilbig, H., Call, J., & Bidmon, H.-J. (2009). Superior olivary complex organization and cytoarchitecture may be correlated with function and catarrhine primate phylogeny. Brain Structure and Function, 213(4-5), 489-497. |
|
Ibbotson, P., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Prototype constructions in early language acquisition. Language and Cognition, 1(1), 59-85. |
|
Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective. Behaviour, 146(7), 979-998. |
|
Kaminski, J., Tempelmann, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs. Developmental Science, 12(6), 831-837. |
|
Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. (2009). Can input explain children's me-for-I errors? Journal of Child Language, 36(5), 1091-1114. |
|
Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). 'I want hold Postman Pat': An investigation into the acquisition of infinitival marker `to'. First Language, 29(3), 313-339. |
|
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 299-314. |
|
Krachun, C., & Call, J. (2009). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) know what can be seen from where. Animal Cognition, 12(2), 317-331. |
|
Krachun, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Can chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) discriminate appearance from reality? Cognition, 112(3), 435-450. |
|
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science, 12(4), 521-535. |
|
Lambach, Y., Herrmann, E., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Physical and social cognition in domestic dogs: A comparative study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 4(2), 49-50. |
|
Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants appreciate the social intention behind a pointing gesture: Commentary on “Children's understanding of communicative intentions in the middle of the second year of life” by T. Aureli, P. Perucchini and M. Genco. Cognitive Development, 24(1), 13-15. |
|
Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science, 12(2), 264-271. |
|
Lieven, E. (2009). Developing constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(1), 191-199. |
|
Lieven, E. (2009). Développement du langage en contexte translinguistique. In Michèle Kail ( |
|
Lieven, E., & Stoll, S. (2009). Language. In Marc H. Bornstein ( |
|
Lieven, E., Salomo, D., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Two-year-old children's production of multiword utterances: A usage-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(3), 481-507. |
|
Liszkowski, U., Schäfer, M., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychological Science, 20(5), 654-660. |
|
Manrique, H. M., Gross, A.-N.-M., & Call, J. (2009). Tool Selection Based on Rigidity in Great Apes. Folia Primatologica, 80(2), 128-128. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., & Call, J. (2009). Assessing Generalization Within and Between Trap Tasks in the Great Apes. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 22(1), 43-60. |
|
Matsui, T., Rakoczy, H., Miura, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Understanding of speaker certainty and false-belief reasoning: a comparison of Japanese and German preschoolers. Developmental Science, 12(4), 602-613. |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(3), 597-624. |
|
McIntyre, M. H., Herrmann, E., Wobber, V., Halbwax, M., Mohamba, C., de Sousa, N., Atencia, R., Cox, D., & Hare, B. (2009). Bonobos have a more human-like second-to-fourth finger length ratio (2D:4D) than chimpanzees: a hypothesized indication of lower prenatal androgens. Journal of Human Evolution, 56(4), 361-365. |
|
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Chimpanzees coordinate in a negotiation game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(6), 381-392. |
|
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2009). The Performance of Bonobos (Pan paniscus), Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in Two Versions of an Object-Choice Task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(3), 304-309. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: I. Introduction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 1-21. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: Ii presenting the dairy method. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 22-31. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: Iii a general description of early verb growth and use. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 32-39. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: Iv pragmatic and semantic flexibility in early verb use. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 40-48. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: V productivity and grammatical flexibility in early verb use. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 49-67. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: Vi differences in early verb growth and use as a function of developmental period, child and verb. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 68-90. |
|
Naigles, L. R., Hoff, E., Vear, D., Tomasello, M., Brandt, S., Waxman, S. R., & Childers, J. B. (2009). Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-n dairy study: Vii general discussion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(2), 91-104. |
|
Narciamdi, J. C. L., & Martin Ordas, G. (2009). La historia como maestra para la investigación en imitación animal. Revista de Historia de la Psicología, 30(2-3), 187-194. |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Eighteen-Month-Old Infants Show Increased Helping Following Priming With Affiliation. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1189-1193. |
|
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Developmental Science, 12(3), F1-F8. |
|
Péle, M., Dufour, V., Thierry, B., & Call, J. (2009). Token transfers among great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes): Species differences, gestural requests, and reciprocal exchange. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(4), 375-384. |
|
Rai, I., Bickel, B., Lieven, E., Banjade, G., Gaenszle, M., Rai, M., Paudyal, N. P., Stoll, S., & Bhatta, T. N. (2009). Mundum: A Case Study of Chintang Ritual Language. In Rila Mukherjee; M. N. Rajesh ( |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2009). Collective intentionality and the roots of human societal life. In Röska-Hardy & Neumann-Held ( |
|
Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Done wrong or said wrong? Young children understand the normative directions of fit of different speech acts. Cognition, 113(2), 205-212. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Brosche, N., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 445-456. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children's selective learning of rule games from reliable and unreliable models. Cognitive Development, 24(1), 61-69. |
|
Rooijakkers, E. F., Kaminski, J., & Call, J. (2009). Comparing dogs and great apes in their ability to visually track object transpositions. Animal Cognition, 12(6), 789-796. |
|
Sanz, C. M., Call, J., & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5(3), 293-296. |
|
Schrauf, C., & Call, J. (2009). Great apes’ performance in discriminating weight and achromatic color. Animal Cognition, 12(4), 567-574. |
|
Seed, A., & Call, J. (2009). Causal knowledge for events and objects in animals. In Shigeru Watanabe; Aaron P. Blaisdell; Ludwig Huber; Allan Young; Keio Gijuku Daigaku., & Centre for Advanced Research on Logic and Sensibility ( |
|
Seed, A., Call, J., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2009). Chimpanzees solve the trap problem when the confound of tool-use is removed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35(1), 23-34. |
|
Seed, A., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2009). Intelligence in Corvids and Apes: A Case of Convergent Evolution? Ethology, 115(5), 401-420. |
|
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Maternal mental state talk and infants' early gestural communication. Journal of Child Language, 36(5), 1053-1074. |
|
Stoll, S., Abbot-Smith, K., & Lieven, E. (2009). Lexically Restricted Utterances in Russian, German, and English Child-Directed Speech. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 75-103. |
|
Tempelmann, S., & Liebal, K. (2009). The Use of Gesture-Sequences in Orang-Utans. Folia Primatologica, 80(2), 144-145. |
|
Tennie, C. (2009). Human culture versus great ape traditions: Mechanisms of observational learning in human children and great apes. PhD Thesis, Univ., Göttingen. |
|
Tennie, C., & Hedwig, D. (2009). How latent solution experiments can help to study differences between human culture and primate traditions. In Emil Potocki and Juliusz Krasinski ( |
|
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1528), 2405-2415. |
|
Tennie, C., Gilby, I. C., & Mundry, R. (2009). The meat-scrap hypothesis: small quantities of meat may promote cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(3), 421-431. |
|
Tennie, C., Glabsch, E., Tempelmann, S., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., & Call, J. (2009). Dogs, Canis familiaris, fail to copy intransitive actions in third-party contextual imitation tasks. Animal Behaviour, 77(6), 1491-1499. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). [Book review] Society need not be selfish. The age of empathy: Nature's lessons for a kinder society / by Frans de Waal. - Random House: 2009. - 320 pp. Nature, 460, 41. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate: based on the 2008 Tanner lectures on human values at Stanford University. Cambridge: MIT Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). Cultural transmission: a view from chimpanzees and human infants. In Ute Schönpflug ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). Postscript: chimpanzee culture. In Kevin N. Laland and Bennett G. Galef ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). The Question of Chimpanzee Culture, plus Postscript (Chimpanzee Culture, 2009). In Kevin N. Laland; Bennett G. Galef ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In Edith L. Bavin ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2009). Universal grammar is dead. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 470-471. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Brandt, S. (2009). Flexibility in the semantics and syntax of children`s early verb use. In L. R. Naigles, E. Hoff, D. Vear, & M. Tomasello ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Kaminski, J. (2009). Like infant, like dog. Science, 325(5945), 1213-1214. |
|
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 534-543. |
|
Warneken, F. (2009). Digging deeper: A response to commentaries on The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 487-490. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Cognition for Culture. In Philip Robbins, Murat Aydede ( |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 455-471. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 397-402. |
|
Wobber, V., & Hare, B. (2009). Testing the social dog hypothesis: Are dogs also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks? Behavioural Processes, 81(3), 423-428. |
|
Wobber, V., Hare, B., Koler-Matznick, J., Wrangham, R. W., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Breed differences in domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris) comprehension of human communicative signals. Interaction Studies, 10(2), 206-224. |
|
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Normativity and context in young children's pretend play. Cognitive Development, 24(2), 146-155. |
|
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children understand multiple pretend identities in their object play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 385-404. |
|
Zimmermann, F., Zemke, F., Call, J., & Gomez, J. C. (2009). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool. Animal Cognition, 12(2), 347-358. |
2008
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Graded representations in the acquisition of English and German transitive constructions. Cognitive Development, 23(1), 48-66. |
|
Abraham, A., Werning, M., Rakoczy, H., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Minds, persons, and space: An fMRI investigation into the relational complexity of higher-order intentionality. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 438-450. |
|
Albiach-Serrano, A., Guillén-Salazar, F., & Call, J. (2008). Inhibition of a Prepotent Response in Mangabeys (Cerocebus Torquatus Lunulatus). Folia Primatologica, 79(5), 306-3. |
|
Amici, F., Aureli, F., & Call, J. (2008). Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Current Biology, 18(18), 1415-1419. |
|
Amici, F., Aureli, F., & Call, J. (2008). Inhibitory Skills Are Associated with High Levels of Fission-Fusion Dynamics. Folia Primatologica, 79(5), 308. |
|
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S. (2008). Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053-1076. |
|
Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Boesch, C., Bearder, S. K., Call, J., Chapman, C. A., Connor, R., Di Fiore, A., Dunbar, R. I. M., Henzi, S. P., Holekamp, K., Korstjens, A. H., Layton, R., Lee, P., Lehmann, J., Manson, J. H., Ramos-Fernandez, G., Strier, K. B., & van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Fission-Fusion Dynamics: New Research Frameworks. Current Anthroplogy, 49(4), 627-654. |
|
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children's repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19(3), 241-248. |
|
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Gräfenhain, M., Liebal, K., Liszkowski, U., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., Warneken, F., & Wyman, E. (2008). Cultural learning and cultural creation. In Müller, U. Carpendale, J. Budwig, N., Sokol, B. ( |
|
Brandt, S., Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The acquisition of German relative clauses: A case study. Journal of Child Language, 35, 325-348. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees do not take into account what others can hear in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 11(1), 175-178. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Inequity Aversion in Great Apes? Folia Primatologica, 79(5), 314. |
|
Buttelmann, D. (2008). Understanding different components in intentional action: How understanding other's mental states helps to learn from others and predict their behaviour. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Behavioral cues that great apes use to forage for hidden food. Animal Cognition, 11(1), 117-128. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rational Tool Use and Tool Choice in Human Infants and Great Apes. Child Development, 79(3), 609-626. |
|
Call, J. (2008). How apes use gestures: The issue of flexibility. In D. K. Oller, & U. Griebel ( |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187-192. |
|
Carpenter, M., & Nielsen, M. (2008). Tools, TV, and trust: Introduction to the special issue on imitation in typically-developing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(4), 225-227. |
|
Chang, F., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Automatic evaluation of syntactic learners in typologically-different languages. Cognitive Systems Research, 9(3), 198-213. |
|
Dąbrowska, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rapid learning of an abstract language-specific category: Polish children's acquisition of the instrumental construction. Journal of Child Language, 35(3), 533-558. |
|
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K. F., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). German Children’s Comprehension of Word Order and Case Marking in Causative Sentences. Child Development, 79(4), 1152-1167. |
|
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K. F., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Young German children's early syntactic competence: a preferential looking study. Developmental Science, 11(4), 575-582. |
|
Fletcher, G. E. (2008). Attending to the outcome of others: disadvantageous inequity aversion in male capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology, 70(9), 901-905. |
|
Girndt, A., Meier, T., & Call, J. (2008). Task Constraints Mask Great Apes' Ability to Solve the Trap-Table Task. Animal Behavior Processes, 34(1), 54-62. |
|
Gräfenhain, M. (2008). Young children's understanding of joint activity. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2008). Chimpanzees infer the location of a reward on the basis of the effect of its weight. Current Biology, 18(9), R370-R372. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2008). Imitation recognition in great apes. Current Biology, 18(7), R288-R290. |
|
Heilbronner, S. R., Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2008). A fruit in the hand or two in the bush? Divergent risk preferences in chimpanzees and bonobos. Biology Letters, 4(3), 246-249. |
|
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Comparing social skills of children and apes: response. Science, 319(5863), 569-570. |
|
Herrmann, E., Wobber, V., & Call, J. (2008). Great apes' (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) understanding of tool functional properties after limited experience. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122(2), 220-230. |
|
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Fair Game for Chimpanzees - Response. Science, 319(5861), 284-284. |
|
Kaminski, J. (2008). The domestic dog: a forgotten star rising again. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(6), 211-212. |
|
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others have seen but not what others believe. Folia Primatologica, 79(5), 344-345. |
|
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109(2), 224-234. |
|
Kaminski, J., Fischer, J., & Call, J. (2008). Prospective object search in dogs: mixed evidence for knowledge of What and Where. Animal Cognition, 11(2), 367-371. |
|
Kirchhofer, K. C. (2008). Comprehension of referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and its motivational constraints. |
|
Liebal, K. (2008). Infants' and young children's understanding of common ground and markedness in communication. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Liebal, K., Colombi, C., Rogers, S. J., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Helping and cooperation in children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 224-238. |
|
Liebal, K., Lausberg, H., Fricke, E., & Müller, C. (2008). Gestural Modes of Representation - a multi-disciplinary approach. In Andrew D.M. Smith, Kenny Smith, Ramon Ferrer i Cancho ( |
|
Lieven, E. (2008). Building Language Competence in First Language Acquisition. European Review, 16(4), 445-456. |
|
Lieven, E. (2008). Learning the English auxiliary: A usage-based approach. In Heike Behrens ( |
|
Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Children's first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In P. Robinson, & N. C. Ellis ( |
|
Liszkowski, U., Albrecht, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Infants' visual and auditory communication when a partner is or is not visually attending. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(2), 157-167. |
|
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732-739. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). How great apes perform in functionally equivalent trap-tube and trap-table tasks. Folia Primatologica, 79(5), 359-360. |
|
Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11(3), 423-430. |
|
Matthews, D., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Grammar. In M. Haith, & J. Benson ( |
|
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 951-962. |
|
Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., & Call, J. (2008). Ape metaphysics: Object individuation without language. Cognition, 106(2), 730-749. |
|
Moll, H., Richter, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Fourteen-Month-Olds Know What “We” Have Shared in a Special Way. Infancy, 13(1), 90-101. |
|
Nielsen, M., & Carpenter, M. (2008). Reflecting on imitation in autism: Introduction to the special issue. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(3), 165-169. |
|
Okamoto-Barth, S., & Call, J. (2008). Tracking and Inferring Spatial Rotation by Children and Great Apes. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 519-543. |
|
Parron, C., Call, J., & Fagot, J. (2008). Behavioural responses to photographs by pictorially naïve baboons (Papio anubis), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioural Processes, 78(3), 351-357. |
|
Pine, J. M., Conti-Ramsden, G., Joseph, K. L., Lieven, E., & Serratrice, L. (2008). Tense over time: testing the Agreement/Tense Omission Model as an account of the pattern of tense-marking provision in early child English. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 55-75. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2008). Pretence as individual and collective intentionality. Mind & Language, 23(5), 499-517. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2008). Taking Fiction Seriously: Young Children Understand the Normative Structure of Joint Pretence Games. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1195-1201. |
|
Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Kollektive Intentionalität und kulturelle Entwicklung. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Philosophie, 56(3), 401-410. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The Sources of Normativity: Young Children's Awareness of the Normative Structure of Games. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 875-881. |
|
Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The early ontogeny of human – dog communication. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 1003-1014. |
|
Russell, Y., Call, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Image scoring in great apes. Behavioural Processes, 78(1), 108-111. |
|
Seed, A., Clayton, N. S., & Emery, N. J. (2008). Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Series B: Biological Sciences, 275(1641), 1421-1429. |
|
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Carpenter, M. (2008). Maternal Talk About Mental States and the Emergence of Joint Visual Attention. Infancy, 13(6), 640-659. |
|
Stephan, A., Lenzen, M., Call, J., & Uhl, M. (2008). Communication and cooperation in living beings and artificial agents. In I. Wachsmuth, M. Lenzen, & G. Knoblich ( |
|
Tennie, C., Hedwig, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). An experimental study of nettle feeding in captive gorillas. American Journal of Primatology, 70(6), 584-593. |
|
Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. (2008). The influence of discourse context on children’s provision of auxiliary BE. American Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 129-158. |
|
Tolar, T. D., Lederberg, A. R., Gokhale, S., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The Development of the Ability to Recognize the Meaning of Iconic Signs. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(2), 71-86. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge: MIT Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2008). How Are Humans Unique? New York Times. Retrieved from http://nyti.ms/1TZlQOr. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2008). Was ist der Mensch(enaffe)? In Detlev Ganten; Jan-Christoph Heilinger; Volker Gerhardt ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2008). Assessing the validity of ape-human comparisons: A reply to Boesch (2007). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122(4), 449-452. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Warneken, F. (2008). Share and share alike. Nature, 454(7208), 1057-1058. |
|
Uher, J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Personality assessment in the Great Apes: Comparing ecologically valid behavior measures, behavior ratings, and adjective ratings. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(4), 821-838. |
|
Uher, J., & Call, J. (2008). How the Great Apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla) Perform on the Reversed Reward Contingency Task II: Transfer to New Quantities, Long-Term Retention, and the Impact of Quantity Ratios. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122(2), 204-212. |
|
Uher, J., Asendorpf, J. B., & Call, J. (2008). Personality in the behaviour of great apes: temporal stability, cross-situational consistency and coherence in response. Animal Behaviour, 75(1), 99-112. |
|
Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not All Emotions Are Created Equal: The Negativity Bias in Social-Emotional Development. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 383-403. |
|
Warneken, F. (2008). Perspectives on de Waal's Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved. Current Anthropology, 49(4), 695-703. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1785-1788. |
|
Wobber, V., Hare, B., & Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Great apes prefer cooked food. Journal of Human Evolution, 55(2), 340-348. |
|
Zube, C., Kleineidam, C. J., Kirschner, S., Neef, J., & Rössler, W. (2008). Organization of the olfactory pathway and odor processing in the antennal lobe of the ant Camponotus floridanus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 506(3), 425-441. |
2007
Albiach-Serrano, A., Guillén-Salazar, F., & Call, J. (2007). Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) solve the reverse contingency task without a modified procedure. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 387-396. |
|
Bickel, B., Banjade, G., Gaenszle, M., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Rai, I. P., Rai, M., Rai, N. K., & Stoll, S. (2007). Free Prefix Ordering in Chintang. Language, 83(1), 43-73. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees Really Know What Others Can See in a Competitive Situation. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 439-448. |
|
Burnham, T. C., & Hare, B. (2007). Engineering Human Cooperation. Human Nature, 18(2), 88-108. |
|
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10(4), F31-F38. |
|
Call, J. (2007). Apes know that hidden objects can affect the orientation of other objects. Cognition, 105(1), 1-25. |
|
Call, J. (2007). Past and present challenges in theory of mind research in nonhuman primates. In C. von Hofsten, & K. Rosander ( |
|
Call, J. (2007). Social knowledge in primates. In L. Barrett, & D. Dunbar ( |
|
Call, J., & Jensen, K. (2007). Chimpanzees may recognize motives and goals, but may not reckon on them. In Empathy and Fairness: Novartis Foundation Symposium 278 (pp. 56-70). New York, NY: Wiley. |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. ( |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Comparing the Gestures of Apes and Monkeys. In Josep Call & Michael Tomasello ( |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The Gestural Repertoire of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In Josep Call & Michael Tomasello ( |
|
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L. (2007). What part of 'no' do children not understand? A usage-based account of multiword negation. Journal of Child Language, 34(2), 251-282. |
|
Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2007). The question of 'what to imitate': inferring goals and intentions from demonstrations. In C. Nehaniv, & K. Dautenhahn ( |
|
Chan, A. (2007). The acquisition of the transitive construction: Methodological and cross-linguistic perspectives. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 677-687. |
|
Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2007). Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): The effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(3), 241-249. |
|
Hare, B. (2007). [Book Review]: Patience reveals chimpanzee cognitive development: Book review of "Cognitive development in chimpanzees" by Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Masaki Tomonaga & Masayuki Tanaka. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1077-1077. |
|
Hare, B. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind: What changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 60-64. |
|
Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., & Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Tolerance Allows Bonobos to Outperform Chimpanzees on a Cooperative Task. Current Biology, 17(7), 619-623. |
|
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317(5843), 1360-1366. |
|
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers in an Ultimatum Game. Science, 318(5847), 107-109. |
|
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 104(32), 12971-12975. |
|
Kidd, E., Brandt, S., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Object relatives made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children's processing of relative clauses. Language & Cognitive Processes, 22(6), 860-897. |
|
Kuehl, H., Williamson, L., Sanz, C. M., Morgan, D., & Boesch, C. (2007). Launch of A.P.E.S. Database. Gorilla Journal, 34, 20-21. |
|
Liebal, K., Müller, C., & Pika, S. ( |
|
Liebal, K., Müller, C., & Pika, S. (2007). Introduction: Gestural communication in nonhuman and human primates. In K. Liebal, & C. Müller ( |
|
Liszkowski, U. (2007). Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and helpfully provide information for a communicative partner. In K. Liebal, C. Müller, & S. Pika ( |
|
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10(2), F1-F7. |
|
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Language, 34(1), 1-20. |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How Toddlers and Preschoolers Learn to Uniquely Identify Referents for Others: A Training Study. Child Development, 78(6), 1744-1759. |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2007). French Children's Use and Correction of Weird Word Orders: A Constructivist Account. Journal of Child Language, 34(2), 381-409. |
|
Mendes, N., Hanus, D., & Call, J. (2007). Raising the level: orangutans use water as a tool. Biology Letters, 3(5), 453-455. |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362(1480), 639-648. |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. Developmental Psychology, 43(2), 309-317. |
|
Moll, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Fourteen-month-olds know what others experience only in joint engagement. Developmental Science, 10(6), 826-835. |
|
Okamoto-Barth, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Great Apes’ Understanding of Other Individuals’ Line of Sight. Psychological Science, 18(5), 462-468. |
|
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The gestural communication of apes. In K. Liebal, C. Müller, & S. Pika ( |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2007). Play, games, and the development of collective intentionality. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 115, 53-67. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2007). Social cognition and social practice. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 33-38. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). "This way!" "No! That way!" - 3-year olds know that two people can have mutually incompatible desires. Cognitive Development, 22(1), 47-68. |
|
Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2007). The Evolutionary Origins of Human Patience: Temporal Preferences in Chimpanzees, Bonobos, and Human Adults. Current Biology, 17(19), 1663-1668. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2007). Cognitive Linguistics and First Language Acquisition. In Dirk Geeraerts and Hubert Cuyckens ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and Communication in the 2nd Year of Life. Child Development Perspectives, 1(1), 8-12. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2007). For Human Eyes Only. New York Times. Retrieved from http://nyti.ms/1OE763d. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2007). If They're So Good at Grammar, Then Why Don't They Talk? Hints From Apes' and Humans' Use of Gestures. Language Learning and Development, 3(2), 133-156. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2007). Ape Gestures and the Origins of Language. In Josep Call & Michael Tomasello ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2007). Introduction: Intention Communication in Nonhuman Primates. In Josep Call & Michael Tomasello ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2007). Introduction: Intentional communication in nonhuman primates. In J. Call, & M. Tomasello ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J.(2007). Wissen Schimpansen, was andere sehen – oder nur wohin sie schauen?. Retrieved from https://www.mpg.de/399309/forschungsSchwerpunkt?c=166422. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121-125. |
|
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A New Look at Infant Pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705-722. |
|
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2007). A szandekok megertese, kozos szandekok a kulturalis gondolkodas gyokerei. Magyar Pszichologiai Szemle, 62(1), 61-105. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11(3), 271-294. |
|
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Spontaneous Altruism by Chimpanzees and Young Children. PLoS Biology, 5(7), 1414-1420. |
|
Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The ontogenetic origins of human cooperation. In R. I. M. Dunbar & Louise Barrett ( |
2006
Abbot-Smith, K., & Behrens, H. (2006). How Known Constructions Influence the Acquisition of Other Constructions: The German Passive and Future Constructions. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 30(6), 995-1026. |
|
Abbot-Smith, K., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. The Linguistic Review, 23, 275-290. |
|
Ambridge, B., Rowland, C. F., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in children's non-subject wh-questions: 'What experimental data can tell us?'. Journal of Child Language, 33(3), 519-557. |
|
Ambridge, B., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The distributed learning effect for children's acquisition of an abstract grammatical construction. Cognitive Development, 21, 174-193. |
|
Barth, J., & Call, J. (2006). Tracking the Displacement of Objects: A Series of Tasks With Great Apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus) and Young Children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32(3), 239-252. |
|
Bertin, E., & Bhatt, R. S. (2006). Three-month-olds' sensitivity to orientation cues in the three-dimensional depth plane. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(1), 45-62. |
|
Bräuer, J. (2006). Perspective taking and food competition in great apes. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London / Series B, 273(1605), 3123-3128. |
|
Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog - casual ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 38-47. |
|
Call, J. (2006). Descartes' two errors: reasoning and reflection from a comparative perspective. In S. Hurley, & M. Nudds ( |
|
Call, J. (2006). Inferences by exclusion in the great apes: the effect of age and species. Animal Cognition, 9(4), 393-403. |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes: gesture communication. In K. Brown ( |
|
Carpenter, M. (2006). Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation. In S. J. Rogers ( |
|
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234-272. |
|
Chen, X., Reid, V. M., & Striano, T. (2006). Oral exploration and reaching toward social and non-social objects in two-, four-, and six-month-old infants. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 1-12. |
|
Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are Nouns Easier to Learn Than Verbs? Three Experimental Studies. In K. Hirsh-Pasek ( |
|
Colmenares, F., Esteban, M. M., & Zaragoza, F. (2006). One-male units and clans in a colony of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas): Effect of male number and clan cohesion on feeding success. American Journal of Primatology, 68(1), 21-37. |
|
Endesfelder, A. (2006). Bilingual first language acquisition: a study on bilingual codemixing. Master Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Behavioral genetics of dog cognition: human-like social skills in dogs are heritable and derived. In Ostrander, Elaine A. ( |
|
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101(3), 495-514. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Call, J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2006). Evolutionary psychology of spatial representations in the Hominidae. Current Biology, 16(17), 1736-1740. |
|
Haun, D. B. M., Rapold, C. J., Call, J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2006). Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(46), 17568-17573. |
|
Helme, A. E., Call, J., Clayton, N. S., & Emery, N. J. (2006). What do bonobos (Pan paniscus) understand about physical contact? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(3), 294-302. |
|
Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., & Colmenares, F. (2006). The utility of generalizability theory in the study of animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 71(4), 983-988. |
|
Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation. Developmental Science, 9(5), 518-529. |
|
Herrmann, E., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task. Animal Cognition, 9(2), 118-130. |
|
Jensen, K., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). What's in it for me?: Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London / Series B, 273(1589), 1013-1021. |
|
Kaminski, J., & Bräuer, J. (2006). Der kluge Hund (Orig.-Ausg.). Reinbek: Rowohlt-Taschenbuch-Verl. |
|
Kaminski, J., & Call, J. (2006). Vokális jelek megértése a border collie kutyafajtánál. Magyar Tudomány, 2, 157-160. |
|
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Goats' behaviour in a competitive food paradigm: evidence for perspective taking? Behaviour, 143(11), 1341-1356. |
|
Kidd, E., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Examining the role of lexical frequency in children's acquisition of sentential complements. Cognitive Development, 21(2), 93-107. |
|
Kirschner, S. (2006). Der Kehlkopf rutscht: Veränderungen lehren das Reden. In Wienecke-Janz, Detlef ( |
|
Kirschner, S. (2006). Klänge und Rhythmen: der Ursprung der Musik. In Wienecke-Janz, Detlef ( |
|
Liebal, K., Pika, S., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture, 6(1), 1-38. |
|
Lieven, E. (2006). How Do Children Develop Syntactic Representations from What They Hear? In P. Vogt, Y. Sugita, E. Tuci, & C. Nehaniv ( |
|
Lieven, E. (2006). Language Development: Overview. In K. Brown ( |
|
Lieven, E. (2006). Producing multiword utterances. In Clark, Eve V.; Kelly, Barbara F. ( |
|
Lieven, E. (2006). Variation in first language acquisition. In K. Brown ( |
|
Liszkowski, U. (2006). Infant Pointing at Twelve Months: Communicative Goals, Motives, and Social-Cognitive Abilities. In Enfield, N.J.; Levinson, Stephen C. ( |
|
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Twelve- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 173-187. |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children's use of referring expression. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(3), 403-422. |
|
McClure, C., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. (2006). Investigating the abstractness of children's early knowledge of argument structure. Journal of Child Language, 33(4), 693-720. |
|
Melis, A. P. (2006). Cooperative and deceptive cognitive skills in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). PhD Thesis, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig. |
|
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(2), 154-162. |
|
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science, 311(5765), 1297-1300. |
|
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72(2), 275-286. |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 603-613. |
|
Moll, H., Koring, C., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Infants determine others' focus of attention by pragmatics and exclusion. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(3), 411-430. |
|
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312(5776), 1038-1040. |
|
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006). How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task. Animal Cognition, 9(3), 193-199. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2006). Pretend play and the development of collective intentionality. Cognitive Systems Research, 7(2-3), 113-127. |
|
Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Two-year-olds' grasp the intentional structure of pretense acts. Developmental Science, 9(6), 558-565. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2006). The role of experience and discourse in children´s developing understanding of pretend play actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 305-335. |
|
Riedel, J., Buttelmann, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food. Animal Cognition, 9(1), 27-35. |
|
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: the persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2(1), 27-50. |
|
Scheumann, M., & Call, J. (2006). Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) and a yellow-cheeked crested gibbon (Nomascus gabriellae) know what is where. International Journal of Primatology, 27(2), 575-602. |
|
Schwier, C., Van Maanen, C., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants. Infancy, 10(3), 303-311. |
|
Striano, T., & Vaish, A. (2006). Seven- to 9-month-old infants use facial expressions to interpret others' actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 753-760. |
|
Striano, T., Henning, A., & Stahl, D. (2006). Sensitivity to interpersonal timing at 3 and 6 months of age. Epigenetic robotics, 251-271. |
|
Striano, T., Henning, A., & Vaish, A. (2006). Selective looking by 12-month-olds to a temporally contingent partner. Interaction Studies, 7(2), 233-250. |
|
Suda, C., & Call, J. (2006). What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apes. Cognition, 99(1), 53-71. |
|
Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2006). Push or pull: emulation versus imitation in great apes and human children. Ethology, 112(12), 1159-1169. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2006). Note of clarification on the coding of light verbs in 'Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax'. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 191-197. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2006). Acquiring linguistic constructions. In William Damon; Richard M. Lerner; Deanna Kuhn; Robert S. Siegler ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2006). Construction Grammar for Kids. Constructions, Special Vol. 1. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2006). Conventions are shared (commentary on Milikan, Language: A biological model). Philosophy of Mind Review, 5, 29-36. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2006). The social-cognitive bases of language development. In K. Brown ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2006). Why don't apes point? In Nicholas J. Enfield; Stephen C. Levinson ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2006). Do chimpanzees know what others see – or only what they are looking at? In Susan Hurley; Matthew Nudds ( |
|
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2006). Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: the cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52(3), 314-320. |
|
Vlamings, P., Uher, J., & Call, J. (2006). How the Great apes (Pan troglodytes Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus and Gorilla gorilla) perform on the reversed contingency task: the effects of food quantity and food visibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32(1), 60-70. |
|
Warneken, F. (2006). The origins of helping and cooperation. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301-1303. |
|
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77(3), 640-663. |
2005
Barrett, H. C., & Behne, T. (2005). Children's understanding of death as the cessation of agency: a test using sleep versus death. Cognition, 96(2), 93-108. |
|
Behne, T. (2005). Developing an understanding of intentions in action and communication. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8(6), 492-499. |
|
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants' understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 328-337. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(2), 145-154. |
|
Call, J. (2005). Chimpanzees are sensitive to some of the psychological states of others. Interaction Studies, 6(3), 413-427. |
|
Call, J. (2005). La evolución de la inteligencia ecológica en los primates: el problema de encontrar y procesar el alimento. In Federico Guillén-Salazar ( |
|
Call, J. (2005). The Self and Other: A Missing Link in Comparative Social Cognition. In H. S. Terrace, & J. Metcalfe ( |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Reasoning and thinking in nonhuman primates. In K. J. Holyoak, & R. G. Morrison ( |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). What Chimpanzees Know about Seeing, Revisited: An Explanation of the Third Kind. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler ( |
|
Call, J., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 151-163. |
|
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8(1), F13-F20. |
|
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253-278. |
|
Dąbrowska, E., & Lieven, E. (2005). Towards a lexically specific grammar of children’s question constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(3), 437-474. |
|
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2005). A new look at the acquisition of relative clauses. Language, 81(4), 882-906. |
|
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Particle placement in early child language: a multifactorial analysis. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 1(1), 89-111. |
|
Gaenszle, M., Bickel, B., Banjade, G., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Rai, I., Rai, M., Rai, N. K., & Stoll, S. (2005). Worshiping the King God: a Preliminary Analysis of Chintang Ritual Language in the Invocation of Rajdeu. In Yogendra P. Yadava ( |
|
Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Infants' electric brain responses to emotional prosody. NeuroReport, 16(16), 1825-1828. |
|
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439-444. |
|
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). The emotional reactivity hypothesis and cognitive evolution: Reply to Miklosi and Topal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 464-465. |
|
Hare, B., Plyusnina, I. Z., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R. W., & Trut, L. N. (2005). Social Cognitive Evolution in Captive Foxes Is a Correlated By-Product of Experimental Domestication. Current Biology, 15(3), 226-230. |
|
Henning, A., Striano, T., & Lieven, E. (2005). Maternal speech to infants at 1 and 3 months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 28(4), 519-536. |
|
Kaminski, J. (2005). Social cognition in different mammalian species: A comparative approach. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69(1), 11-18. |
|
Kemp, N., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young Children's Knowledge of the "Determiner" and "Adjective" Categories. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(3), 592-609. |
|
Liebal, K., Müller, C., & Pika, S. (2005). Introduction: Gestural communication in nonhuman and human primates. Gestural Communication in Nonhuman and Human Primates: Special issue of Gesture, 1-5. |
|
Liszkowski, U. (2005). Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and helpfully provide information for a communicative partner. Gesture, 5(1-2), 135-154. |
|
Lohmann, H., Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2005). Guessing versus choosing - and seeing versus believing - in false belief tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 451-469. |
|
Lohmann, H., Tomasello, M., & Meyer, S. (2005). Linguistic Communication and Social Understanding. In J. W. Astington, & J. A. Baird ( |
|
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2005). The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cognitive Development, 20(1), 121-136. |
|
Moll, H. (2005). Infants' understanding of perceiving, attending, and knowing. PhD Thesis, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig. |
|
Mulcahy, N. J., Call, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005). Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(1), 23-32. |
|
Pika, S., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): Repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 65(1), 39-61. |
|
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Gestural communication of apes. Gestural Communication in Nonhuman and Human Primates: Special issue of Gesture, 41-56. |
|
Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L. (2005). Testing the Agreement/Tense Omission Model: Why the data on children's use of non-nominative 3psg subjects count against the ATOM. Journal of Child Language, 32(2), 269-289. |
|
Rai, N. K., Bickel, B., Banjade, G., Gaenszle, M., Lieven, E., Paudyal, N. P., Rai, I., Rai, M., & Stoll, S. (2005). Triplication and ideophones in Chintang. In Yogendra P. Yadava ( |
|
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). How Children Turn Objects into Symbols: A Cultural Learning Account. In Namy, L. ( |
|
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). On tools and toys: how children learn to act on and pretend with 'virgin objects'. Developmental Science, 8(1), 57-72. |
|
Reid, V. M., & Striano, T. (2005). Adult gaze influences infant attention and object processing: implications for cognitive neuroscience. European Journal of Neuroscience: European Neuroscience Association, 21(6), 1763-1766. |
|
Reid, V. M., & Striano, T. (2005). Review of "Brain development and cognition: A reader". Journal of Child Language, 32(2), 469-472. |
|
Rohland, N., Pollack, J. L., Nagel, D., Beauval, C., Airvaux, J., Pääbo, S., & Hofreiter, M. (2005). The population history of extant and extinct hyenas. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 22(12), 2435-2443. |
|
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L. (2005). The incidence of error in young children's wh-questions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 384-404. |
|
Schirmer, A., Striano, T., & Friederic, A. D. (2005). Sex differences in the preattentive processing of vocal emotional expressions. NeuroReport, 16(6), 635-639. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/neuroreport/Abstract/2005/04250/Sex_differences_in_the_preattentive_processing_of.24.aspx. |
|
Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2005). Coordinated affect with mothers and strangers: A longitudinal analysis of joint engagement between 5 and 9 months of age. Cognition & Emotion, 19(5), 781-790. |
|
Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2005). Social-cognitive skills between 5 and 10 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(Part 4), 559-568. |
|
Striano, T., & Bushnell, E. W. (2005). Haptic perception of material properties by 3-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 28(3), 266-289. |
|
Striano, T., & Liszkowski, U. (2005). Sensitivity to the context of facial expression in the still face at 3-, 6-, and 9-months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 28(1), 10-19. |
|
Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science, 8(4), 333-343. |
|
Striano, T., Henning, A., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to social contingencies between 1 and 3 months of age. Developmental Science, 8(6), 509-518. |
|
Suda, C., & Call, J. (2005). Piagetian conservation of discrete quantities in bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Cognition, 8(4), 220-235. |
|
Theakston, A. L., & Lieven, E. (2005). The acquisition of auxiliaries BE and HAVE: an elicitation study. Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 587-616. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2005). The acquisition of auxiliary syntax: BE and HAVE. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1), 247-277. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2005). Beyond formalities: The case of language acquisition. The Linguistic Review, 22(2-4), 183-197. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2005). Uniquely human cognition is a product of human culture. In S. C. Levinson, & P. Jaisson ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Boston, Mass.: Blackwell Publishing. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). Intention reading and imitative learning. In S. Hurley, & N. Chater ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Slobin, D. I. ( |
|
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). In search of the uniquely human - Response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 721-727. |
|
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675-691. |
|
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). German-speaking children's productivity with syntactic constructions and case morphology: Local cues act locally. First Language, 25(1), 103-125. |
|
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young children's sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(4), 541-558. |
2004
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Training 2;6-year-olds to produce the transitive construction: the role of frequency, semantic similarity and shared syntactic distribution. Developmental Science, 7(1), 48-55. |
|
Aitken, N. C., Smith, S., Schwarz, C., & Morin, P. A. (2004). Single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) discovery in mammals: a targeted-gene approach. Molecular Ecology, 13(6), 1423-1431. |
|
Barth, J., Povinelli, D. J., & Cant, J. G. H. (2004). Bodily origins of the self. In Behrend, Douglas A.; Beike, Denise R.; Lampinen, James M. ( |
|
Barth, J., Reaux, J. E., & Povinelli, D. J. (2004). Chimpanzees'(Pan troglodytes) use of gaze cues in object-choice tasks: different methods yield different results. Animal Cognition, 8(2), 84-92. |
|
Belke, T. W., Pierce, W. D., & Jensen, K. (2004). Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel. Behavioural Processes, 67(1), 1-10. |
|
Bertin, E., & Bhatt, R. S. (2004). The Thatcher illusion and face processing in infancy. Developmental Science, 7(4), 431-436. |
|
Brandt, S. (2004). [Book review] Gunther Kress & Theo van Leeuwen, Multimodal discourse: The modes and media of contemporary communication. Language in Society, 33(1), 115-118. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 88(3-4), 299-317. |
|
Call, J. (2004). [Book review]: Is there only one way to become sapiens?: [Book review of] How Homo became sapiens: on the evolution of thinking by Peter Gärdenfors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(6), 247-249. |
|
Call, J. (2004). Inferences About the Location of Food in the Great Apes (Pan panicus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(2), 232-241. |
|
Call, J. (2004). Social intelligence. In Bernard Thierry, Mewa Singh and Werner Kaumanns ( |
|
Call, J. (2004). The Use of Social Information in Chimpanzees and Dogs. In Rogers, Lesley; Kaplan, Gisela ( |
|
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 'Unwilling' versus 'unable': chimpanzees' understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488-498. |
|
Chen, X., Striano, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2004). Auditory-oral matching behavior in newborns. Developmental Science, 7(1), 42-47. |
|
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J., Beehner, J., Bergman, T., Johnson, S. E., Kitchen, D. M., Palombit, R. A., Rendall, D., & Silk, J. B. (2004). Factors Affecting Reproduction and Mortality Among Baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. International Journal of Primatology, 25(2), 401-428. |
|
Eckardt, W., & Zuberbühler, K. (2004). Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 15(3), 400-411. |
|
Fischer, J. (2004). Emergence of individual recognition in young macaques. Animal Behaviour, 67(4), 655-661. |
|
Fischer, J., Call, J., & Kaminski, J. (2004). A pluralistic account of word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 481-481. |
|
Fischer, J., Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2004). Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56(2), 140-148. |
|
Hare, B. (2004). Using comparative studies of primate and canid social cognition to model our Miocene minds. PhD Thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts. |
|
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68(3), 571-581. |
|
Henning, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Early sensitivity to interpersonal timing. In L. Berthouze, H. Kozima, C. G. Prince, G. Sandini, G. Stojanov, G. Metta, & C. Balkenius ( |
|
Hofreiter, M., Rabeder, G., Jänicke-Després, V. R., Withalm, G., Nagel, D., Paunovic, M., Jambresic, G., & Pääbo, S. (2004). Evidence for reproductive isolation between cave bear populations. Current Biology, 14(1), 40-43. |
|
Hofreiter, M., Serre, D., Rohland, N., Rabeder, G., Nagel, D., Conard, N. J., Münzel, S., & Pääbo, S. (2004). Lack of phylogeography in European mammals before the last glaciation. PNAS, 101(35), 12963-12968. |
|
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping". Science, 304(5677), 1682-1683. |
|
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Body orientation and face orientation: two factors controlling apes' begging behavior from humans. Animal Cognition, 7(4), 216-223. |
|
Kidd, E. (2004). Grammars, parsers, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 31(2), 480-483. |
|
Liebal, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 64(4), 377-396. |
|
Liebal, K., Pika, S., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Social communication in siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): Use of gestures and facial expressions. Primates, 45(1), 41-57. |
|
Liebal, K., Pika, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). To move or not to move: How apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interaction Studies, 5(2), 199-219. |
|
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3), 297-307. |
|
Maslen, R. J. C., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2004). A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1319-1333. |
|
Mersmann, D. (2004). Social learning in domestic dogs (Canis Familiaris). Diploma Thesis, Univ., Wien. |
|
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barrier. Developmental Science, 7(1), F1-F9. |
|
Namy, L. L., Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2004). The Changing Role of Iconicity in Non-Verbal Symbol Learning: A U-Shaped Trajectory in the Acquisition of Arbitrary Gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5(1), 37-57. |
|
Okamoto, S., Tanaka, M., & Tomonaga, M. (2004). Looking back: The "representational mechanism" of joint attention in an infant chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Japanese Psychological Research, 46(3), 236-245. |
|
Rabeder, G., Hofreiter, M., Nagel, D., & Withalm, G. (2004). New Taxa of Alpine Cave Bears (Ursidae, Carnivora). Cahiers scientifiques. Hors série, 2, 49-67. |
|
Rakoczy, H. (2004). The development of performing and understanding pretend play: a cultural learning perspective. PhD Thesis, Universität Leipzig, Leipzig. |
|
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending: A test of the "behaving-as-if" construal of children's early concept of pretense. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 388-399. |
|
Range, F., & Fischer, J. (2004). Vocal Repertoire of Sooty Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys) in the Taï National Park. Ethology, 110(4), 301-321. |
|
Reid, V. M., Striano, T., Kaufmann, J., & Johnson, M. E. (2004). Eye gaze cueing facilitates neural processing of objects in 4-month-old infants. NeuroReport, 15(16), 2553-2555. |
|
Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Morgan, R. (2004). Who is Doing What to Whom? Young Infants' Developing Sense of Social Causality in Animated Displays. Perception, 33(3), 355-369. |
|
Savage, C., & Lieven, E. (2004). Can the usage-based approach to language development be applied to analysis of developmental stuttering? Stammering Research, 1(2), 83-100. |
|
Scheumann, M., & Call, J. (2004). The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Animal Cognition, 7(4), 224-230. |
|
Smith, S., Aitken, N. C., Schwarz, C., & Morin, P. A. (2004). Characterization of 15 single nucleotide polymorphism markers for chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Molecular Ecology Notes, 4(3), 348-351. |
|
Striano, T. (2004). Direction of regard and the still-face effect in the first year: Does intention matter? Child Development, 75(2), 468-479. |
|
Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2004). Contribution of facial and vocal cues in the still-face by 4 month old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(4), 499-508. |
|
Suda, C. (2004). Piagetian conservation in the great apes. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Suda, C., & Call, J. (2004). Piagetian Liquid Conservation in the Great Apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(3), 265-279. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31(1), 61-99. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2004). Learning through others. Daedalus, 133(1), 51-58. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2004). Review: Human language and our reptilian brain: The subcortical bases of speech, syntax, and thought by Philip Lieberman. Language, 80(2), 325-327. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2004). Syntax or semantics? Response to Lidz et al. Cognition, 93(2), 139-140. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2004). What kind of evidence could refute the UG hypothesis?: Commentary on Wunderlich. Studies in Language, 28(3), 642-645. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2004). Zwei Hypothesen zur Primatenkognition. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Philosophie, 52, 585-601. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2004). The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited. Animal Cognition, 7(4), 213-215. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling children´s spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language, 31(01), 101-121. |
|
Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Vocal versus facial cues in social referencing. Developmental Science, 7(3), 261-269. |
2003
Barth, J., & Call, J. (2003). Great apes performance in a delayed response task: The effect of movement and mental distraction. Folia Primatologica, 74(4), 184. |
|
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). All great apes can follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Folia Primatologica, 74(4), 184-185. |
|
Call, J. (2003). Beyond learning fixed rules and social cues: abstraction in the social arena. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1435), 1189-1196. |
|
Call, J. (2003). Spatial rotations and transpositions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 44(4), 347-357. |
|
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2003). On imitation in apes and children. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 26(3), 325-349. |
|
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Social cognition. In Maestripieri, D. ( |
|
Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(3), 257-263. |
|
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27(6), 843-873. |
|
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). Response to Silvio Loddo´s Commentary. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(5), 547-549. |
|
Chang, F., Bock, K., & Goldberg, A. (2003). Can thematic roles leave traces of their places? Cognition, 90(1), 29-49. |
|
Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Children extend both words and non-verbal actions to novel exemplars. Developmental Science, 6(2), 185-190. |
|
Diessel, H. (2003). The relationship between demonstratives and interrogatives. Studies in Language, 27(3), 635-655. |
|
Fischer, J. ( |
|
Fischer, J. (2003). [Book review] Tree of Origin: What Primate Behaviour Can Tell Us about Human Social Evolution by F. B. De Waal (ed.). Ethology, 109(10), 861-862. |
|
Hanus, D., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Quantity-based judgments by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Folia primatologica, 74(4), 196-197. |
|
Hare, B., Adessi, E., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Visalberghi, E. (2003). Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, know what conspecifics do and do not see? Animal Behaviour, 65(1), 131-142. |
|
Kidd, E. (2003). Relative clause comprehension revisited: commentary on Eisenberg (2002). Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 671-679. |
|
Kitano, T., Schwarz, C., Nickel, B., & Pääbo, S. (2003). Gene diversity patterns at 10 X-chromosomal loci in humans and chimpanzees. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 20(8), 1281-1289. |
|
Kitchen, D. M., Seyfarth, R. M., Fischer, J., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53(6), 374-384. |
|
Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Social communication in captive siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): Use of gestures and facial expressions. Folia Primatologica, 74(4), 205-206. |
|
Lieven, E., Behrens, H., Speares, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Early syntactic creativity: a usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 30(2), 333-370. |
|
Lohmann, H. (2003). Shaping views and changing minds: How linguistic communication influences children's understanding of false beliefs. PhD Thesis, Univ., Leipzig. |
|
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Language and Social Understanding: Commentary on Nelson et al. Human Development, 46(1), 47-50. |
|
Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The Role of Language in the Development of False Belief Understanding: A Training Study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130-1144. |
|
Meyer, S., & von Haeseler, A. (2003). Identifying site-specific substitution rates. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 20(2), 182-189. |
|
Pika, S. (2003). The gestural communication of subadult gorillas (Gorilla Gorilla) and subadult bonobos (Pan Paniscus) in captivity. PhD Thesis, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Münster. |
|
Pika, S., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60(3), 95-111. |
|
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of acquisition order in wh-questions: re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 609-635. |
|
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children´s linguistic representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 557-567. |
|
Striano, T., Rochat, P., & Legerstee, M. (2003). The role of modelling and request type on symbolic comprehension of objects and gestures in young children. Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 27-45. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The Role of the Input in the Acquisition of Third Person Singular Verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(4), 863-877. |
|
Tomasello, M. ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2003). Introduction: Some surprises for Psychologists. In Tomasello, Michael ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2003). On the Different Origins of Symbols and Grammar. In Language evolution (pp. 94-110). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2003). The Key is Social Cognition. In Gentner, Dedre ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (2003). What paradox? A response to Naigles (2002). Cognition, 88(3), 317-323. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding Attention: 12- and 18-Month-Olds Know What Is New for Other Persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906-912. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What Makes Human Cognition Unique? From Individual to Shared to Collective Intentionality. Mind & Language, 18(2), 121-147. |
|
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states - the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 153-156. |
|
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees versus humans: it´s not that simple. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(6), 239-240. |
|
Vlamings, P., Barth, J., & Call, J. (2003). Inhibition in great apes: Performance on a detour-reaching task. Folia Primatologica, 74(4), 227. |
2002
Bradley, B. J., Doran, D., Robbins, M. M., Williamson, E., Boesch, C., & Vigilant, L. (2002). Comparative analyses of genetic social structure in wild gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) using DNA from feces and hair. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 117(Suppl. 34), 47-48. |
|
Call, J. (2002). A fish-eye lens for comparative studies: Broadening the scope of animal cognition. Animal Cognition, 5(1), 15-16. |
|
Call, J. (2002). Social Knowledge and Social Manipulation in Monkeys and Apes. In Harcourt, C.; Sherwood, B. ( |
|
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of information in social learning. In Dautenhahn, K. ( |
|
Call, J., Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Postconflict third-party affiliation in stumptailed macaques. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 209-216. |
|
Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2002). The chemistry of social learning: Commentary on Want & Harris (2002). Developmental Science, 5(1), 22-24. |
|
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). A new false belief test for 36-month-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(3), 393-420. |
|
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Understanding 'prior intentions' enables 2-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. Child Development, 73(5), 1431-1441. |
|
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism and developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91-106. |
|
Chang, F. (2002). Symbolically speaking: A connectionist model of sentence production. Cognitive Science, 26(5), 609-651. |
|
Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions from massed or distributed exposures. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 967-978. |
|
Ebersberger, I., Metzler, D., Schwarz, C., & Pääbo, S. (2002). Genomewide comparison of DNA sequences between humans and chimpanzees. American Journal of Human Genetics, 70(6), 1490-1497. |
|
Enard, W., Ebersberger, I., Fischer, A., Heißig, F., Hellmann, I., Höffner, B., Khaitovich, P., Kitano, T., Köhler, K., Metzler, D., Nickel, B., Przeworski, M., Schwarz, C., Nowick, K., Wiebe, V., Winkler, M., Zöllner, S., & Pääbo, S.(2002). Functional genomics in the chimpanzee. |
|
Fischer, J. (2002). Developmental Modifications in the Vocal Behavior of Non-Human Primates. In Ghazanfar, Asif A. ( |
|
Fischer, J., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2002). An overview of the Barbary macaque, Macaca sylvanus, vocal repertoire. Folia Primatologica, 73(1), 32-45. |
|
Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2002). Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: Influences of context, age, and individuality. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111(3), 1465-1474. |
|
Hare, B., & Wrangham, R. W. (2002). Integrating two evolutionary models for the study of social cognition. In Bekoff, Marc; Allen, Colin; Burghardt, Gordon M. ( |
|
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1634-1636. |
|
Hofreiter, M., Capelli, C., Krings, M., Waits, L., Conard, N. J., Munzel, S., Rabeder, G., Nagel, D., Paunovic, M., Jambresic, G., Meyer, S., Weiss, G., & Pääbo, S. (2002). Ancient DNA analyses reveal high mitochondrial DNA sequence diversity and parallel morphological evolution of late pleistocene cave bears. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 19(8), 1244-1250. |
|
Israel, M. (2002). Literally speaking. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 423-432. |
|
Pika, S., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Gestural communication in gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) in captivity: A comparison. Advances in Ethology, 37, 65. |
|
Rabeder, G., Hofreiter, M., Nagel, D., Pääbo, S., & Withalm, G. (2002). Die neue Taxonomy der Höhlenbären. In W. Rosendahl, M. Morgan, & M. Correa ( |
|
Roberts, L. D. (2002). How words have content: an explanatory hypothesis based on developmental psychology. Language & Communication, 22(1), 83-106. |
|
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Differential effects of happy, neutral, and sad still face on 2-, 4- and 6-month-old infants. Infant & Child Development, 11(4), 289-303. |
|
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Who´s in the mirror? Child Development, 73(1), 35-46. |
|
Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Infant Physical and Social Cognition. In Smelser, N. J.; Baltes, Paul B. ( |
|
Striano, T., Brennan, P. A., & Vanman, E. J. (2002). Maternal depressive symptoms and 6-month-old infants' sensitivity to facial expressions. Infancy, 3(1), 115-126. |
|
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2002). Going, going, gone: the acquisition of the verb 'go'. Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 783-811. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2002). [Book review] Not waving but speaking: How important were gestures in the evolution of language? Book reviewed 'From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language' by Michael C. Corballis. Nature, 417(6891), 791-792. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2002). Some facts about primate (including human) social learning and communication. In Cangelosi, A., & Parisi, D. ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2002). The emergence of grammar in early child language. In Givón, T. ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2002). Things are what they do: Katherine Nelson's functional approach to language and cognition. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3(1), 5-19. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2002). A tale of two theories: Response to Fisher. Cognition, 83(2), 207-214. |
|
Tomasello, M., & Zuberbühler, K. (2002). Primate vocal and gestural communication. In M. Bekoff ( |
|
Wittek, A. (2002). Learning the meaning of change-of-state verbs: a case study of German child language. Berlin: Mouton de Druyter. |
|
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2002). German children's productivity with tense morphology: The Perfekt (present perfect). Journal of Child Language, 29(3), 567-589. |
|
Zuberbühler, K. (2002). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63(2), 293-299. |
2001
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2001). What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders. Cognitive Development, 16(2), 679-692. |
|
Baltes, P. B., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Infant physical and social cognition. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences: Human Developments (pp. 7410-7414). Amsterdam: Elsevier. |
|
Behrens, H., & Wittek, A. (2001). Research Activities on the Acquisition of German. Child Language Bulletin, 21. |
|
Call, J. (2001). Body imitation in an enculturated Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Cybernetics and Systems, 32(1-2), 97-119. |
|
Call, J. (2001). Chimpanzee social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(9), 388-393. |
|
Call, J. (2001). Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(2), 159-171. |
|
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2001). Do apes and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition, 3(4), 207-220. |
|
Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of English dative constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(2), 253-267. |
|
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others' intentions in children with autism and children with developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589-599. |
|
Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 739-748. |
|
Diessel, H. (2001). The ordering distribution of main and adverbial clauses: A typological study. Language, 77(3), 433-455. |
|
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2001). The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A corpus-based analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 97-142. |
|
Fischer, J., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2001). Functional referents and acoustic similarity revisited: the case of Barbary macaque alarm calls. Animal Cognition, 4(1), 29-35. |
|
Fischer, J., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2001). The alarm calls of Barbary macaques: a new look. Folia Primatologica, 72(3), 160. |
|
Fischer, J., Hammerschmidt, K., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2001). Acoustic features of female chacma baboon barks. Ethology, 107(1), 33-54. |
|
Fischer, J., Metz, M., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2001). Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 925-931. |
|
Fischer, J., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2001). 'Vocal antlers': The acoustic structure of male baboon wahoos. Advances in Ethology, 36. |
|
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know and do not know? Animal Behaviour, 61(1), 139-151. |
|
Lloyd, J., Lieven, E., & Arnold, P. (2001). Oral conversations between hearing-impaired children and their normally hearing peers and teachers. First Language, 21(61), 83-107. |
|
Pika, S., & Tomasello, M. (2001). 'Separating the wheat from the chaff': A novel food processing technique in captive gorillas. Primates, 42(2), 167-170. |
|
Seres, M., Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). Successful formation of a large chimpanzee group out of two preexisting subgroups. Zoo Biology, 20(6), 501-515. |
|
Striano, T. (2001). From social expectations to social cognition in early infancy. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 65(3), 361-370. |
|
Striano, T., & Thoermer, C. (2001). Function and Determinants of Oral Probing in Early Infancy. In J. v. d. Kamp ( |
|
Striano, T., Tomasello, M., & Rochat, P. (2001). Social and object support for early symbolic play. Developmental Science, 4(4), 442-455. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2001). Bruner on language acquisition. In D. Bakhurst, & S. Shanker ( |
|
Tomasello, M. (2001). Could we please lose the mapping metaphor, please? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(6), 1119-1120. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2001). Cultural transmission: A view from chimpanzees and human infants. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(2), 135-146. |
|
Tomasello, M. (2001). Evolution of human cognition. In N. J. Smelser, & P. B. Baltes ( |
|
Tomasello, M., & Bates, E. ( |
|
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Fogleman, T. (2001). The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta. Animal Behaviour, 61(2), 335-343. |
|
Tschudin, A., Call, J., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harris, G., & van der Elst, C. (2001). Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(1), 100-105. |